Categorical Scaling of Duration Bisection in Pigeons (Columba livia), Mice (Mus musculus), and Humans (Homo sapiens)

A fundamental assumption underlying research in translational neuroscience is that animal models represent many of the same neurocognitive mechanisms and decision processes used by humans. Clear demonstrations of such correspondences will be crucial to the discovery of the neurobiological underpinnings of higher-level cognition. One domain likely to support fruitful comparisons is interval timing, because humans and other animals appear to share basic similarities in their ability to discriminate the durations of events in the seconds-to-minutes range. Here, we report that in a duration-bisection procedure using a series of anchor durations ranging from 2 through 5 s, pigeon, mouse, and human subjects classified a given signal duration as subjectively shorter than an adjacent, physically shorter signal duration when the two durations lay on opposite sides of a putative category boundary. These bisection reversals provide strong evidence for continuity of temporal cognition across a wide range of vertebrate species.

[1]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  L. Allan,et al.  Temporal bisection with trial referents , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  Catalin V. Buhusi,et al.  What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[4]  S. Harnad Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition , 1990 .

[5]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Human bisection at the geometric mean , 1991 .

[6]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Absolute identification by relative judgment. , 2005, Psychological review.

[7]  J. E. Weaver,et al.  Pigeons group time intervals according to their relative duration , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  Catalin V Buhusi,et al.  Interval timing with gaps and distracters: evaluation of the ambiguity, switch, and time-sharing hypotheses. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  J. Gibbon On the form and location of the Psychometric Bisection Function for time , 1981 .

[10]  R. Ragot,et al.  Auditory/visual duration bisection in patients with left or right medial-temporal lobe resection , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[11]  J G Fetterman,et al.  Categorical scaling of time: implications for clock-counter models. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[12]  M. Hauser,et al.  Spontaneous number discrimination of multi-format auditory stimuli in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) , 2002, Cognition.

[13]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Identification and bisection of temporal durations and tone frequencies: common models for temporal and nontemporal stimuli. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  R. Church,et al.  Bisection of temporal intervals. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[15]  Catalin V Buhusi,et al.  Temporal integration as a function of signal and gap intensity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and pigeons (Columba livia). , 2002, Journal of comparative psychology.

[16]  W. Meck,et al.  Common Representations of Abstract Quantities , 2007 .

[17]  W. Meck Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[18]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Sources of variability and systematic error in mouse timing behavior. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[19]  J. J. Higa,et al.  Interval timing in Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[20]  S. Fairhurst,et al.  Scalar timing in animals and humans , 2002 .

[21]  Peter R. Killeen,et al.  Categorical scaling of time: implications for clock-counter models. , 1995 .

[22]  David F. Sherry,et al.  Interval Timing by an Invertebrate, the Bumble Bee Bombus impatiens , 2006, Current Biology.

[23]  S. Harnad Psychophysical and cognitive aspects of categorical perception: A critical overview , 1987 .

[24]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[25]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Interval-timing deficits in individuals at high risk for schizophrenia , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[26]  W. Meck,et al.  Sensory modality and time perception in children and adults , 2007, Behavioural Processes.

[27]  W H Meck,et al.  Paying Attention to Time as one Gets Older , 2001, Psychological science.

[28]  A. Machado,et al.  Testing the scalar expectancy theory (SET) and the learning-to-time model (LeT) in a double bisection task , 2005, Learning & behavior.

[29]  W H Meck,et al.  The use of timing behaviors in animals and humans to detect drug and/or toxicant effects. , 1999, Neurotoxicology and teratology.

[30]  Jonathan Henderson,et al.  Timing in Free-Living Rufous Hummingbirds, Selasphorus rufus , 2006, Current Biology.

[31]  L. Allan The Location and Interpretation of the Bisection Point , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[32]  J. Wearden,et al.  Scalar Properties in Animal Timing: Conformity and Violations , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  J. Wearden,et al.  Stimulus range effects in temporal bisection by humans. , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[34]  D A Stubbs,et al.  Scaling of stimulus duration by pigeons. , 1976, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[35]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Modality-specific circadian rhythmicities influence mechanisms of attention and memory for interval timing , 1991 .

[36]  Neil Stewart,et al.  Similarity and dissimilarity as evidence in perceptual categorization , 2005 .

[37]  A. Santi,et al.  Pigeons’ timing of an arbitrary and a naturalistic auditory stimulus: tone versus cooing , 2001, Behavioural Processes.

[38]  Fuat Balci,et al.  Cross-domain transfer of quantitative discriminations: Is it all a matter of proportion? , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[39]  J. Wearden,et al.  Stimulus Spacing Effects in Temporal Bisection by Humans , 1995 .

[40]  A Kacelnik,et al.  Relative Importance of Perceptual and Mnemonic Variance in Human Temporal Bisection , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[41]  J. Wearden,et al.  Scalar Properties in Human Timing: Conformity and Violations , 2006 .

[42]  Temporal integration as a function of signal and gap intensity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and pigeons (Columba livia). , 2002 .

[43]  S. Hanson,et al.  Learned Categorical Perception in Neural Nets: Implications for Symbol Grounding , 1995 .

[44]  J H Wearden,et al.  Categorical scaling of stimulus duration by humans. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.