Categorical Scaling of Duration Bisection in Pigeons (Columba livia), Mice (Mus musculus), and Humans (Homo sapiens)
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Gibbon,et al. Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] L. Allan,et al. Temporal bisection with trial referents , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[3] Catalin V. Buhusi,et al. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[4] S. Harnad. Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition , 1990 .
[5] J. Gibbon,et al. Human bisection at the geometric mean , 1991 .
[6] Gordon D. A. Brown,et al. Absolute identification by relative judgment. , 2005, Psychological review.
[7] J. E. Weaver,et al. Pigeons group time intervals according to their relative duration , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[8] Catalin V Buhusi,et al. Interval timing with gaps and distracters: evaluation of the ambiguity, switch, and time-sharing hypotheses. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[9] J. Gibbon. On the form and location of the Psychometric Bisection Function for time , 1981 .
[10] R. Ragot,et al. Auditory/visual duration bisection in patients with left or right medial-temporal lobe resection , 2005, Brain and Cognition.
[11] J G Fetterman,et al. Categorical scaling of time: implications for clock-counter models. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[12] M. Hauser,et al. Spontaneous number discrimination of multi-format auditory stimuli in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) , 2002, Cognition.
[13] Gordon D. A. Brown,et al. Identification and bisection of temporal durations and tone frequencies: common models for temporal and nontemporal stimuli. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] R. Church,et al. Bisection of temporal intervals. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[15] Catalin V Buhusi,et al. Temporal integration as a function of signal and gap intensity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and pigeons (Columba livia). , 2002, Journal of comparative psychology.
[16] W. Meck,et al. Common Representations of Abstract Quantities , 2007 .
[17] W. Meck. Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[18] C. Gallistel,et al. Sources of variability and systematic error in mouse timing behavior. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[19] J. J. Higa,et al. Interval timing in Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) , 2004, Behavioural Processes.
[20] S. Fairhurst,et al. Scalar timing in animals and humans , 2002 .
[21] Peter R. Killeen,et al. Categorical scaling of time: implications for clock-counter models. , 1995 .
[22] David F. Sherry,et al. Interval Timing by an Invertebrate, the Bumble Bee Bombus impatiens , 2006, Current Biology.
[23] S. Harnad. Psychophysical and cognitive aspects of categorical perception: A critical overview , 1987 .
[24] J. Gibbon. Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .
[25] J. Gibbon,et al. Interval-timing deficits in individuals at high risk for schizophrenia , 2005, Brain and Cognition.
[26] W. Meck,et al. Sensory modality and time perception in children and adults , 2007, Behavioural Processes.
[27] W H Meck,et al. Paying Attention to Time as one Gets Older , 2001, Psychological science.
[28] A. Machado,et al. Testing the scalar expectancy theory (SET) and the learning-to-time model (LeT) in a double bisection task , 2005, Learning & behavior.
[29] W H Meck,et al. The use of timing behaviors in animals and humans to detect drug and/or toxicant effects. , 1999, Neurotoxicology and teratology.
[30] Jonathan Henderson,et al. Timing in Free-Living Rufous Hummingbirds, Selasphorus rufus , 2006, Current Biology.
[31] L. Allan. The Location and Interpretation of the Bisection Point , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[32] J. Wearden,et al. Scalar Properties in Animal Timing: Conformity and Violations , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[33] J. Wearden,et al. Stimulus range effects in temporal bisection by humans. , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[34] D A Stubbs,et al. Scaling of stimulus duration by pigeons. , 1976, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[35] Warren H. Meck,et al. Modality-specific circadian rhythmicities influence mechanisms of attention and memory for interval timing , 1991 .
[36] Neil Stewart,et al. Similarity and dissimilarity as evidence in perceptual categorization , 2005 .
[37] A. Santi,et al. Pigeons’ timing of an arbitrary and a naturalistic auditory stimulus: tone versus cooing , 2001, Behavioural Processes.
[38] Fuat Balci,et al. Cross-domain transfer of quantitative discriminations: Is it all a matter of proportion? , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[39] J. Wearden,et al. Stimulus Spacing Effects in Temporal Bisection by Humans , 1995 .
[40] A Kacelnik,et al. Relative Importance of Perceptual and Mnemonic Variance in Human Temporal Bisection , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[41] J. Wearden,et al. Scalar Properties in Human Timing: Conformity and Violations , 2006 .
[42] Temporal integration as a function of signal and gap intensity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and pigeons (Columba livia). , 2002 .
[43] S. Hanson,et al. Learned Categorical Perception in Neural Nets: Implications for Symbol Grounding , 1995 .
[44] J H Wearden,et al. Categorical scaling of stimulus duration by humans. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.