Size invariance in visual object priming

The magnitude of priming resulting from perception of a briefly presented picture of an object in an earlier trial block, as assessed by naming reaction times (RTs), was independent of whether the primed object was presented at the same or a different size as when originally viewed. RTs and error rates for "same" responses for old-new shape judgments were much increased by a change in object size from initial presentation. We conjecture that this dissociation between the effects of size consistency on naming and old-new shape recognition may reflect the differential functioning of 2 independent systems subserving object memory: 1 for representing object shape and the other for representing its size, position, and orientation (metric attributes). Allowing for response selection, object naming RTs may provide a relatively pure measure of the functioning of the shape system. Both the shape and metric systems may affect the feelings of familiarity that govern old-new episodic shape judgments. A comparison of speeded naming and episodic recognition judgments may provide a behavioral, noninvasive technique for determining the neural loci of these 2 systems. The "recognition" latency of a shape originally viewed at one size is lengthened when that shape is presented at a different size (Jolicoeur, 1987). This result, and a number of others demonstrating time costs for comparing two simultaneously (or immediately sequential) presented shapes that differ in size (Besner, 1983; Bundesen & Larsen, 1975; Ellis, Allport, Humphreys, & Collis, 1989; Jolicoeur & Besner, 1987; Larsen, 1985; Larsen & Bundesen, 1978) have led to the belief that shapes are stored at a specified scale (Kosslyn, 1987; Ullman, 1989).' To achieve matching of the representation of that shape when it is presented at a different size, according to this view, scaling transformations are necessary (e.g., Ullman, 1989). Presumably, the additional time to perform these transformations is the reason why the match of a different-size shape requires more time. Recognition was placed in quotes in the previous paragraph because, curiously, the effects of size consistency on a relatively direct measure of object classification, basic-level naming, have never been assessed. Jolicoeur's (1987) measure of recognition was old-new discrimination. Jolicoeur himself cautioned that it was possible that the effects of size inconsistency in his experiment were not reflecting the perceptual representation used for pattern recognition of the object but rather the processes for episodic discrimination. We report here that Jolicoeur's disquiet was well founded: Size consistency does not affect basic-level naming reaction times (RTs). We used a picture-primin g task where it has been well documented that an object viewed on one occasion

[1]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[2]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Speed of size scaling in human vision , 1972 .

[3]  Dave Bartram,et al.  The role of visual and semantic codes in object naming , 1974 .

[4]  Richard C. Atkinson,et al.  Search and decision processes in recognition memory. , 1974 .

[5]  C. Bundesen,et al.  Visual transformation of size. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  P. Suppes,et al.  Contemporary Developments in Mathematical Psychology , 1976 .

[7]  C. Bundesen,et al.  Size scaling in visual pattern recognition. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Maryanne Martin Local and global processing: The role of sparsity , 1979 .

[9]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[10]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .

[11]  M. Kubovy,et al.  Does pattern matching require the normalization of size and orientation? , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  L. Jacoby,et al.  On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[14]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[15]  J. R. Pomerantz Global and local precedence: selective attention in form and motion perception. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  D. Besner Visual Pattern Recognition: Size Preprocessing Re-Examined , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[17]  P. A. Kolers,et al.  Procedures of mind. , 1984 .

[18]  A. Larsen,et al.  Pattern matching: Effects of size ratio, angular difference in orientation, and familiarity , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[20]  D. Schacter Implicit memory: History and current status. , 1987 .

[21]  P. Jolicoeur A size-congruency effect in memory for visual shape , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[22]  Rob Ellis,et al.  Multiple levels of representation for visual objects: a behavioural study , 1987 .

[23]  M. Mishkin,et al.  The anatomy of memory. , 1987, Scientific American.

[24]  P Jolicoeur,et al.  Additivity and interaction between size ratio and response category in the comparison of size-discrepant shapes. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  M. Perenin,et al.  Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  S. Kosslyn Aspects of a cognitive neuroscience of mental imagery. , 1988, Science.

[27]  S. Ullman Aligning pictorial descriptions: An approach to object recognition , 1989, Cognition.

[28]  L. Brooks,et al.  Episodic effects on picture identification: implications for theories of concept learning and theories of memory. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Varieties of Object Constancy , 1989, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[30]  Michael J. Tarr,et al.  Orientation dependence in three-dimensional object recognition , 1989 .

[31]  Eileen Kowler Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes. , 1990, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[32]  J. O'Regan Eye movements and reading. , 1990, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[33]  D. Schacter,et al.  Priming of Nonverbal Information and the Nature of Implicit Memory , 1990 .

[34]  I. Biederman,et al.  Evidence for Complete Translational and Reflectional Invariance in Visual Object Priming , 1991, Perception.

[35]  I. Biederman,et al.  Priming contour-deleted images: Evidence for intermediate representations in visual object recognition , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[37]  I. Biederman,et al.  Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition. , 1992, Psychological review.

[38]  J. R. Thomas,et al.  Artificial intelligence and its applications , 1987 .

[39]  M. Manosevitz,et al.  High-Speed Scanning in Human Memory , 2022 .