Improving Understanding of Health-Relevant Numerical Information

In this chapter, we discuss why risks are often not communicated in a transparent and understandable way and why this is problematic. At the core of the chapter are four examples that illustrate how risk communication can be improved. These examples are (a) the use of natural frequencies in the context of diagnostic reasoning, (b) the use of visual aids to support the beneficial effect of natural frequency representations, (c) the use of natural frequencies to clarify the distinction between relative and absolute risk reduction, and (d) a clarification of the meaning and pitfalls of survival rates that are often used to quantify the benefit of screening programs. In each of these topics, we describe original empirical studies illuminating a specific problem as well as how these problems can be overcome, and we discuss practical implications of the results and the proposed solutions. Subsequently, we illustrate, using an example from mammography screening, what transparent risk communication could look like. The chapter concludes with a discussion of training programs designed to enhance health-related, high-stakes decision making.

[1]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Communicating Statistical Information , 2000, Science.

[2]  P. Gtzsche,et al.  Breast Cancer Screening in Denmark , 2017, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[3]  D. Krantz,et al.  The effects of statistical training on thinking about everyday problems , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  J. Hanson Shared decision making: have we missed the obvious? , 2008, Archives of internal medicine.

[5]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  How to Improve Bayesian Reasoning Without Instruction: Frequency Formats , 1995 .

[6]  David M Studdert,et al.  Defensive medicine among high-risk specialist physicians in a volatile malpractice environment. , 2005, JAMA.

[7]  C. K. Mertz,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Numeracy and Decision Making , 2022 .

[8]  H. Welch,et al.  Likelihood that a woman with screen-detected breast cancer has had her "life saved" by that screening. , 2011, Archives of internal medicine.

[9]  G. Haas,et al.  The worldwide epidemiology of prostate cancer: perspectives from autopsy studies. , 2008, The Canadian journal of urology.

[10]  Gideon Keren,et al.  The Wiley Blackwell handbook of judgement and decision making , 2015 .

[11]  U. Hoffrage,et al.  Chances and risks in medical risk communication , 2015, German medical science : GMS e-journal.

[12]  Greg Brooks,et al.  A systematic review of controlled trials evaluating interventions in adult literacy and numeracy , 2005 .

[13]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Representation facilitates reasoning: what natural frequencies are and what they are not , 2002, Cognition.

[14]  J. Schulkin,et al.  Numerical Reasoning in Judgments and Decision Making about Health , 2016 .

[15]  E. Murray,et al.  'A heartbeat moment': qualitative study of GP views of patients bringing health information from the internet to a consultation. , 2010, The British journal of general practice : the journal of the Royal College of General Practitioners.

[16]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Science Current Directions in Psychological , 2010 .

[17]  Katherine L. Milkman,et al.  A User's Guide to Debiasing , 2014 .

[18]  W. Gaissmaier,et al.  Numbers can be worth a thousand pictures: individual differences in understanding graphical and numerical representations of health-related information. , 2012, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[19]  P. Ubel,et al.  Reducing the Influence of Anecdotal Reasoning on People’s Health Care Decisions: Is a Picture Worth a Thousand Statistics? , 2005, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[20]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTEREST Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics , 2022 .

[21]  B. Zikmund‐Fisher,et al.  The Effect of Format on Parents' Understanding of the Risks and Benefits of Clinical Research: A Comparison Between Text, Tables, and Graphics , 2010, Journal of health communication.

[22]  Carey K. Morewedge,et al.  Debiasing Decisions , 2015 .

[23]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Measuring Risk Literacy: The Berlin Numeracy Test , 2012, Judgment and Decision Making.

[24]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Simple tools for understanding risks: from innumeracy to insight , 2003, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[25]  H. Arkes Costs and benefits of judgment errors: Implications for debiasing. , 1991 .

[26]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Less is more: Overdiagnosis and overtreatment: evaluation of what physicians tell their patients about screening harms. , 2013, JAMA internal medicine.

[27]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Numerical Reasoning in Judgments and Decision Making about Health: Measuring numeracy , 2014 .

[28]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  Numbers needed to decide. , 2009, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[29]  Ulrich Hoffrage,et al.  Teaching Bayesian reasoning: an evaluation of a classroom tutorial for medical students , 2002, Medical teacher.

[30]  Ulrich Hoffrage,et al.  Visual representation of statistical information improves diagnostic inferences in doctors and their patients. , 2013, Social science & medicine.

[31]  W. Gaissmaier,et al.  Deceiving Numbers , 2011, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[32]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Visual aids improve diagnostic inferences and metacognitive judgment calibration , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[33]  L. Schwartz,et al.  Participation in mammography screening , 2007, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[34]  I. Lipkus Numeric, Verbal, and Visual Formats of Conveying Health Risks: Suggested Best Practices and Future Recommendations , 2007, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[35]  Francesca Vicentini,et al.  Football cultural events and stock market returns: the case of FIFA World Cup , 2016 .

[36]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Better Doctors, Better Patients, Better Decisions: Envisioning Health Care 2020 , 2011 .

[37]  Steven Woloshin,et al.  Using a Drug Facts Box to Communicate Drug Benefits and Harms Two Randomized Trials , 2009, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[38]  J. Skolbekken,et al.  Alternative definitions , 1998, BMJ.

[39]  Mike Pearson,et al.  Visualizing Uncertainty About the Future , 2022 .

[40]  D. Mechanic,et al.  Concepts of trust among patients with serious illness. , 2000, Social science & medicine.

[41]  Steven Woloshin,et al.  The Drug Facts Box: Providing Consumers with Simple Tabular Data on Drug Benefit and Harm , 2007, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[42]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Effective communication of risks to young adults: using message framing and visual aids to increase condom use and STD screening. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[43]  D. Michaels Regarding "Phenylpropanolamine and hemorrhagic stroke in the hemorrhagic stroke project": mercenary epidemiology--data reanalysis and reinterpretation for sponsors with financial interest in the outcome. , 2006, Annals of epidemiology.

[44]  Asha K. Jitendra,et al.  The Effects of Instruction in Solving Mathematical Word Problems for Students with Learning Problems , 1999 .

[45]  Ulrich Hoffrage,et al.  Designing Risk Communication in Health , 2012 .

[46]  Brian Butterworth The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance: Mathematical Expertise , 2006 .

[47]  H. D. de Koning,et al.  Quality-of-life effects of prostate-specific antigen screening. , 2012, The New England journal of medicine.

[48]  P. Ubel,et al.  Making numbers matter: present and future research in risk communication. , 2007, American journal of health behavior.

[49]  G Gigerenzer,et al.  Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences , 1998, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[50]  D. Eddy Judgment under uncertainty: Probabilistic reasoning in clinical medicine: Problems and opportunities , 1982 .

[51]  Felix G. Rebitschek,et al.  A Simple Tool for Communicating the Benefits and Harms of Health Interventions , 2016, MDM policy & practice.

[52]  Erika A. Waters,et al.  Reducing aversion to side effects in preventive medical treatment decisions. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[53]  Amit Kumar Ghosh,et al.  Translating evidence-based information into effective risk communication: current challenges and opportunities. , 2005, The Journal of laboratory and clinical medicine.

[54]  P. Budetti,et al.  Tort reform and the patient safety movement: seeking common ground. , 2005, JAMA.

[55]  C. Hennekens,et al.  Phenylpropanolamine and hemorrhagic stroke in the Hemorrhagic Stroke Project: a reappraisal in the context of science, the Food and Drug Administration, and the law. , 2006, Annals of Epidemiology.

[56]  Paul Starr The social transformation of American medicine , 1983 .

[57]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Calculated Risks: How to Know When Numbers Deceive You , 2002 .

[58]  P. Gøtzsche,et al.  Informed choice requires information about both benefits and harms , 2009, Journal of Medical Ethics.