Post-Error Behavioral Adjustments Are Facilitated by Activation and Suppression of Task-Relevant and Task-Irrelevant Information Processing

Error monitoring by the posterior medial frontal cortex (pMFC) has been linked to post-error behavioral adaptation effects and cognitive control dynamics in lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC). It remains unknown, however, whether control adjustments following errors produce post-error behavioral adjustments (PEBAs) by inhibiting inappropriate responses or facilitating goal-directed ones. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to investigate the hemodynamic correlates of PEBAs in a stimulus–response compatibility task. Our task was designed to test whether PEBAs are implemented by suppressing motor responses primed by irrelevant stimulus features (face location), redirecting attention to relevant features (face gender), or both or neither of these possibilities. Independent of PEBAs, error-related pMFC activation was followed by post-error recruitment of prefrontal and parietal control regions and, crucially, both (1) suppressed response-related activity in sensorimotor cortex and (2) enhanced target processing in face-sensitive sensory cortex (“fusiform face area”). More importantly, by investigating the covariation between post-error hemodynamic activity and individual differences in PEBAs, we showed that modulation of task-related motor and sensory processing was dependent on whether participants produced generally slower responses (“post-error slowing”; PES) or selectively reduced interference effects (“post-error reduction of interference”; PERI), respectively. Each of these behaviorally dependent effects was mediated by distinct LPFC control mechanisms (PES: inferior frontal junction; PERI: superior frontal sulcus). While establishing relationships between PEBAs and cognitive control, our findings suggest that the neural architecture underlying sequential behavioral adaptation may be determined primarily by how control is executed by the individual when adjustments are needed.

[1]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  Hugh Garavan,et al.  Individual differences in error processing: a review and reanalysis of three event-related fMRI studies using the GO/NOGO task. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Donald Laming,et al.  Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .

[4]  Wim Fias,et al.  Post-error slowing: An orienting account , 2009, Cognition.

[5]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[6]  C. Carter,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[9]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[10]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Neuroimaging of Performance Monitoring: Error Detection and Beyond , 2004, Cortex.

[11]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A General Statistical Analysis for fMRI Data , 2000, NeuroImage.

[12]  Hartmut Leuthold,et al.  Control over response priming in visuomotor processing: a lateralized event-related potential study , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  A. Schnitzler,et al.  Magnetic stimulation of the dorsal premotor cortex modulates the Simon effect. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[14]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Bryan Rodgers,et al.  What does a Man do after he Makes an Error? An Analysis of Response Programming , 1977 .

[16]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  J. R. Simon The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .

[18]  K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  Alcohol Consumption Impairs Detection of Performance Errors in Mediofrontal Cortex , 2002, Science.

[19]  T. Egner Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[20]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[21]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Avoiding another mistake: Error and posterror neural activity associated with adaptive posterror behavior change , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Perceptual Learning, Motor Learning and Automaticity Switching from Automatic to Controlled Behavior: Cortico-basal Ganglia Mechanisms , 2022 .

[23]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[24]  T. Egner Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  H. Garavan,et al.  Dissociable Executive Functions in the Dynamic Control of Behavior: Inhibition, Error Detection, and Correction , 2002, NeuroImage.

[26]  R. Compton,et al.  Alpha power is influenced by performance errors. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[27]  Richard Ridderinkhof Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.

[28]  M. Brass,et al.  Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: Meta‐analyses of switching and Stroop studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[29]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[30]  W. Gehring,et al.  Functions of the Medial Frontal Cortex in the Processing of Conflict and Errors , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Norbert Kathmann,et al.  Neural correlates of error awareness , 2007, NeuroImage.

[33]  W. Hulstijn,et al.  Drug-induced stimulation and suppression of action monitoring in healthy volunteers , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[34]  G Lohmann,et al.  LIPSIA--a new software system for the evaluation of functional magnetic resonance images of the human brain. , 2001, Computerized medical imaging and graphics : the official journal of the Computerized Medical Imaging Society.

[35]  M. Farah,et al.  Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control? , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  R. Proctor,et al.  The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[37]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[38]  Joseph M. Orr,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex makes 2 contributions to minimizing distraction. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  J. Tanji,et al.  Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in executive behavioral control. , 2008, Physiological reviews.

[40]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain , 2007, NeuroImage.

[41]  C. Kennard,et al.  Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[42]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[43]  Hartmut Leuthold,et al.  Short article: Control over speeded actions: A common processing locus for micro- and macro-trade-offs? , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[44]  Josep Marco-Pallarés,et al.  Neural Mechanisms Underlying Adaptive Actions after Slips , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Functional organization of the medial frontal cortex , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[46]  James R Müller,et al.  Microstimulation of the superior colliculus focuses attention without moving the eyes. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[47]  M. Brass,et al.  The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  M. Rushworth Intention, Choice, and the Medial Frontal Cortex , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[49]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Unconscious Errors Enhance Prefrontal-Occipital Oscillatory Synchrony , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[51]  Axel Mecklinger,et al.  Picture database of morphed faces (MoFa) : technical report , 2005 .

[52]  R. West,et al.  Tracking the temporal dynamics of updating cognitive control: an examination of error processing. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[53]  Wim Fias,et al.  Outcome expectancy and not accuracy determines posterror slowing: ERP support , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[54]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[55]  S. Brandt,et al.  Executive control over response priming and conflict: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  T. Braver,et al.  Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. , 2007 .

[57]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Learned Predictions of Error Likelihood in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Science.

[58]  Todd S. Braver,et al.  A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions , 2006, Neurocomputing.

[59]  Marty G. Woldorff,et al.  Momentary reductions of attention permit greater processing of irrelevant stimuli , 2009, NeuroImage.

[60]  M. Ullsperger,et al.  ERP correlates of error relevance , 2004 .

[61]  B. Forstmann,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of action control: resisting the call of the Sirens. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[62]  Robert F. Simons,et al.  Oops!.. I did it again: An ERP and behavioral study of double-errors , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[63]  D. Laming Choice reaction performance following an error , 1979 .

[64]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[65]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[66]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  A Preference for Contralateral Stimuli in Human Object- and Face-Selective Cortex , 2007, PloS one.

[67]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[68]  Hiroki M. Morimoto,et al.  Functional dissociation in right inferior frontal cortex during performance of go/no-go task. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[69]  E. Soetens,et al.  Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 2002, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

[70]  Stefan Pollmann,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex contributions to attention control. , 2004, Experimental psychology.

[71]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[72]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Attention Enhances the Neural Processing of Relevant Features and Suppresses the Processing of Irrelevant Features in Humans: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of the Stroop Task , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.