Written Language as a Window in to Residual Language Deficits: A Study of Children With Persistent and Residual Speech and Language Impairments

Previous work has suggested that, because writing is a late-acquired and complex skill, it may be a particularly sensitive index of language difficulties in children. Evidence in support of this view was obtained in a study contrasting 161 normally-developing control children aged from 7.5 to 13 years with 75 twin children of the same age who either had specific speech-language impairments, or were co-twins of affected children. Written narratives were elicited from children using a sequence of five photographs depicting a simple story, and were analysed for grammatical complexity and accuracy, intelligibility, and semantic content. Only 42 of the twins could spell well enough to attempt the narrative task. Some co-twins of affected children had deficits in written language, despite normal performance on oral language tests. Most children with language impairments were poor at writing, with particularly marked deficits on a measure of spelling and punctuation. Children with language impairments made a relatively high proportion of phonologically inaccurate spelling errors when compared with younger children at a similar vocabulary level. Those who did poorly on a nonword repetition test were especially likely to have poor written language. However, four children with pure speech difficulties produced age-appropriate written narratives.

[1]  H. Catts,et al.  The relationship between speech-language impairments and reading disabilities. , 1993, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[2]  D. Bishop Genetic and environmental risks for specific language impairment in children. , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[3]  L. Shriberg,et al.  Phonological disorders III: a procedure for assessing severity of involvement. , 1982, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[4]  Susan E. Stothard,et al.  Is preschool language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? , 2000, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[5]  H. Taylor,et al.  Academic outcomes in children with histories of speech sound disorders. , 2000, Journal of communication disorders.

[6]  J. Tomblin,et al.  Language Basis of Reading and Reading Disabilities: Evidence From a Longitudinal Investigation , 1999 .

[7]  G. Conti-Ramsden,et al.  Follow-up of children attending infant language units: outcomes at 11 years of age. , 2001, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[8]  A D Baddeley,et al.  The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition: a test of phonological working memory. , 1994, Memory.

[9]  C. Norbury,et al.  Phonological processing, language, and literacy: a comparison of children with mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss and those with specific language impairment. , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[10]  G. Levi,et al.  Language Disorders and Prognosis for Reading Disabilities in Developmental Age , 1982, Perceptual and motor skills.

[11]  H. Catts,et al.  Toward an understanding of developmental language and reading disorders. , 1986, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[12]  J. Tomblin,et al.  Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. , 1997, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[13]  D V Bishop,et al.  Language-impaired 4-year-olds: distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. , 1987, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[14]  J. Marshall,et al.  Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach , 1973 .

[15]  B A Lewis,et al.  Residual effects of preschool phonology disorders in grade school, adolescence, and adulthood. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[16]  G. Conti-Ramsden,et al.  Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). , 2001, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[17]  D V Bishop,et al.  Phonological awareness and literacy development in children with expressive phonological impairments. , 1995, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[18]  D. Bishop Genetic influences on language impairment and literacy problems in children: same or different? , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[19]  C Donlan,et al.  Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. , 1996, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[20]  Susan E. Stothard,et al.  Language-impaired preschoolers: a follow-up into adolescence. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[21]  Dorothy V. M. Bishop,et al.  Uncommon Understanding: Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children , 1997 .

[22]  Virginia W. Berninger,et al.  Reading and Writing Acquisition , 1996 .

[23]  H. Catts,et al.  Phonological and spatial processing abilities in language- and reading-impaired children. , 1988, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[24]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? ☆ , 1990 .

[25]  J H Hogben,et al.  On the "specifics" of specific reading disability and specific language impairment. , 2000, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[26]  B. Lewis Pedigree Analysis of Children with Phonology Disorders , 1992, Journal of learning disabilities.

[27]  D. Bishop,et al.  Accurate non-word spelling despite congenital inability to speak: phoneme-grapheme conversion does not require subvocal articulation. , 1989, British journal of psychology.

[28]  G. Butterworth,et al.  A LONGITUDINAL STUDY USING THE WPPSI AND WISC‐R WITH AN ENGLISH SAMPLE , 1979 .

[29]  D. Bishop,et al.  A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. , 1990, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[30]  P. Wolff,et al.  Family patterns of developmental dyslexia. Part III: Spelling errors as behavioral phenotype. , 1996, American journal of medical genetics.

[31]  S. Sasanuma,et al.  Recovery and plasticity in child-onset aphasics: Ultimate outcome at adulthood , 1990 .

[32]  D. Abercrombie,et al.  The Edinburgh articulation test , 1971 .

[33]  R. Gillam,et al.  Phonological Awareness Training and Short‐Term Working Memory: Clinical Implications , 1996 .

[34]  C Donlan,et al.  GENETIC BASIS OF SPECIFIC LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT: EVIDENCE FROM A TWIN STUDY , 1995, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[35]  M. Snowling,et al.  Is dyslexia a form of specific language impairment? a comparison of dyslexic and language impaired children as adolescents , 2000, Annals of dyslexia.

[36]  Joystackhouse An Investigation of Reading and Spelling Performance in Speech Disordered Children , 1982 .

[37]  Doris J. Johnson,et al.  Written narratives of normal and learning disabled children , 1989, Annals of dyslexia.

[38]  L. Rescorla,et al.  Do late-talking toddlers turn out to have reading difficulties a decade later? , 2000, Annals of dyslexia.

[39]  J. Carlisle An exploratory study of morphological errors in children's written stories , 1996 .

[40]  F Lhermitte,et al.  Acquired aphasia in children. , 1965, Brain : a journal of neurology.