Phenomenology-Friendly Neuroscience: The Return To Merleau-Ponty As Psychologist

This paper reports on the Kuhnian revolution now occurring in neuropsychology that is finally supportive of and friendly to phenomenology – the “enactive” approach to the mind-body relation, grounded in the notion of self-organization, which is consistent with Husserl and Merleau-Ponty on virtually every point. According to the enactive approach, human minds understand the world by virtue of the ways our bodies can act relative to it, or the ways we can imagine acting. This requires that action be distinguished from passivity, that the mental be approached from a first person perspective, and that the cognitive capacities of the brain be grounded in the emotional and motivational processes that guide action and anticipate action affordances. It avoids the old intractable problems inherent in the computationalist approaches of twentieth century atomism and radical empiricism, and again allows phenomenology to bridge to neuropsychology in the way Merleau-Ponty was already doing over half a century ago.

[1]  Maurice Merleau-Ponty The structure of behavior , 1963 .

[2]  M Corbetta,et al.  Attentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color, and velocity in humans. , 1990, Science.

[3]  An ontology of consciousness , 1986 .

[4]  G. Rhodes,et al.  The Simon Then Garfunkel Effect: Semantic Priming, Sensitivity, and the Modularity of Face Recognition , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  Natika Newton Visualizing is imagining seeing: a reply to White , 1989 .

[6]  M. Farah The neural basis of mental imagery , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[7]  Frederick J. Wertz Cognitive Psychology and the Understanding of Perception , 1987 .

[8]  Ralph D. Ellis,et al.  Curious Emotions: Roots of consciousness and personality in motivated action , 2005 .

[9]  A. Milner,et al.  Neuropsychology of Consciousness , 2019, Frontiers Research Topics.

[10]  A. Luria Higher Cortical Functions in Man , 1980, Springer US.

[11]  R. Restak The Brain , 1979 .

[12]  Natika Newton EXPERIENCE AND IMAGERY , 1982 .

[13]  The psychology of imagination , 1949 .

[14]  R. Ellis,et al.  Three paradoxes of phenomenal consciousness: Bridging the explanatory gap , 1998 .

[15]  L. Foster,et al.  Experience and Theory , 1970 .

[16]  T. Natsoulas What is wrong with appendage theory of consciousness , 1993 .

[17]  Shaun Gallagher,et al.  The self in contextualized action , 1999 .

[18]  P. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Focussing in reasoning and decision making , 1993, Cognition.

[19]  G. E. Smith The Human Brain , 1924, Nature.

[20]  S. Runeson,et al.  Constant velocity — Not perceived as such , 1974, Psychological research.

[21]  Eugene T. Gendlin,et al.  Experiencing and the Creation of Meaning , 1962 .

[22]  D. Chalmers Facing Up to the Problem of Consciousness , 1995 .

[23]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Imagery and Cognition , 1991 .

[24]  Conscious Emotion in a Dynamic System: How I Can Know How I Feel , 2000 .

[25]  J. Richardson Imagery and the Brain , 1991 .

[26]  M. Alexander,et al.  Principles of Neural Science , 1981 .

[27]  E. Gendlin The primacy of the body, not the primacy of perception , 1992 .

[28]  Jonathan Winson,et al.  Brain and psyche , 1985 .

[29]  F. Hesse,et al.  Emotional influences on semantic priming. , 1993, Cognition & emotion.

[30]  A. Damasio,et al.  Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  J. Schmahmann,et al.  The function of the cerebellum in cognition, affect and consciousness: Empirical support for the embodied mind , 2001 .

[32]  R. Srebro Localization of visually evoked cortical activity in humans. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  Stuart A. Kauffman,et al.  ORIGINS OF ORDER , 2019, Origins of Order.

[34]  Masao Ito Movement and thought: identical control mechanisms by the cerebellum , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[35]  R. Ellis Questioning Consciousness: The interplay of imagery, cognition, and emotion in the human brain , 1995 .

[36]  A. Caramazza Cognitive Neuropsychology and Neurolinguistics: Advances in Models of Cognitive Function and Impairment , 1990 .

[37]  E. Lavy,et al.  Cognitive avoidance and attentional bias: Causal relationships , 1994, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[38]  A. Bahill,et al.  Learning to track predictable target waveforms without a time delay. , 1985, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[39]  Jonathan Cole,et al.  The Man Who Tasted Shapes , 2003 .

[40]  M. Sheets‐Johnstone The Primacy of Movement , 1999 .

[41]  M. Jouvet Neurophysiology of the states of sleep. , 1967, Physiological reviews.

[42]  M. Raichle,et al.  The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[43]  S. Luper The Absurdity of Life , 1992 .

[44]  Natika Newton The sensorimotor theory of cognition , 1993 .

[45]  CONSCIOUSNESS, QUALIA, AND REENTRANT SIGNALLING , 1991 .

[46]  F. Jackson What Mary Didn't Know , 1986 .

[47]  R. Ellis,et al.  The Caldron of Consciousness: Motivation, Affect and Self-Organization- An Anthology. Advances in Consciousness Research , 2000 .

[48]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  Natika Newton Dennett on intrinsic intentionality , 1992 .

[50]  M. Posner,et al.  Attentional Mechanisms and Conscious Experience , 1992 .

[51]  Natika Newton,et al.  Foundations of understanding , 1996 .

[52]  J. Allman,et al.  The Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[53]  Maurice Merleau-Ponty Phenomenology of Perception , 1964 .

[54]  C. Aurell Man's Triune Conscious Mind , 1989, Perceptual and motor skills.

[55]  Jacques Monod,et al.  Chance and Necessity , 1970 .

[56]  G. Logan Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.