Zebra finches can use positional and transitional cues to distinguish vocal element strings

Learning sequences is of great importance to humans and non-human animals. Many motor and mental actions, such as singing in birds and speech processing in humans, rely on sequential learning. At least two mechanisms are considered to be involved in such learning. The chaining theory proposes that learning of sequences relies on memorizing the transitions between adjacent items, while the positional theory suggests that learners encode the items according to their ordinal position in the sequence. Positional learning is assumed to dominate sequential learning. However, human infants exposed to a string of speech sounds can learn transitional (chaining) cues. So far, it is not clear whether birds, an increasingly important model for examining vocal processing, can do this. In this study we use a Go-Nogo design to examine whether zebra finches can use transitional cues to distinguish artificially constructed strings of song elements. Zebra finches were trained with sequences differing in transitional and positional information and next tested with novel strings sharing positional and transitional similarities with the training strings. The results show that they can attend to both transitional and positional cues and that their sequential coding strategies can be biased toward transitional cues depending on the learning context. This article is part of a Special Issue entitled: In Honor of Jerry Hogan.

[1]  Carel ten Cate,et al.  On the phonetic and syntactic processing abilities of birds: From songs to speech and artificial grammars , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[2]  H. Williams,et al.  Syllable chunking in zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) song. , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.

[3]  E. Newport,et al.  Computation of Conditional Probability Statistics by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1998 .

[4]  D. Amit,et al.  Memory of Ordinal Number Categories in Macaque Monkeys , 2006 .

[5]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Sequential learning in non-human primates , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  R. Zann The Zebra Finch: A Synthesis of Field and Laboratory Studies , 1996 .

[7]  Carel ten Cate,et al.  Rule learning by zebra finches in an artificial grammar learning task: which rule? , 2012, Animal Cognition.

[8]  R. Henson Short-Term Memory for Serial Order: The Start-End Model , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  Marina Nespor,et al.  Perceptual and memory constraints on language acquisition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Birdsong, Speech, and Language: Exploring the Evolution of Mind and Brain , 2013 .

[11]  C. Scharff,et al.  Twitter evolution: converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  Elisabetta Versace,et al.  The apes’ edge: positional learning in chimpanzees and humans , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[13]  Timothy Q. Gentner,et al.  Working memory for patterned sequences of auditory objects in a songbird , 2010, Cognition.

[14]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Serial Expertise of Rhesus Macaques , 2003, Psychological science.

[15]  Herbert S. Terrace,et al.  The Phylogeny and Ontogeny of Serial Memory: List Learning by Pigeons and Monkeys , 1993 .

[16]  M. Hauser,et al.  Segmentation of the speech stream in a non-human primate: statistical learning in cotton-top tamarins , 2001, Cognition.

[17]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  Macaque monkeys categorize images by their ordinal number , 2000, Nature.

[18]  H. S. Terrace,et al.  Chunking by a pigeon in a serial learning task , 1987, Nature.

[19]  M. H. Kelly,et al.  Domain-general abilities applied to domain-specific tasks: Sensitivity to probabilities in perception, cognition, and language☆ , 1994 .

[20]  J. B. Trobalon,et al.  Statistical computations over a speech stream in a rodent , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  Gabriel J. L. Beckers,et al.  Zebra finches exhibit speaker-independent phonetic perception of human speech , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[22]  Michael Colombo,et al.  A positional coding mechanism in pigeons after learning multiple three-item lists , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[23]  C. ten Cate,et al.  Song discrimination learning in zebra finches induces highly divergent responses to novel songs , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  Graham J. Hitch,et al.  How is the serial order of a verbal sequence coded? Some comparisons between models , 2005, Memory.

[25]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[26]  Willem H. Zuidema,et al.  Simple rules can explain discrimination of putative recursive syntactic structures by a songbird species , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Richard N. Henson,et al.  Serial order in short-term memory , 2001 .

[28]  Gary F. Marcus,et al.  Stepwise acquisition of vocal combinatorial capacity in songbirds and human infants , 2013, Nature.