Order Matters: Sequencing Scale-Realistic Versus Simplified Models to Improve Science Learning

Teachers choosing between different models to facilitate students’ understanding of an abstract system must decide whether to adopt a model that is simplified and striking or one that is realistic and complex. Only recently have instructional technologies enabled teachers and learners to change presentations swiftly and to provide for learning based on multiple models, thus giving rise to questions about the order of presentation. Using disjoint individual growth modeling to examine the learning of astronomical concepts using a simulation of the solar system on tablets for 152 high school students (age 15), the authors detect both a model effect and an order effect in the use of the Orrery, a simplified model that exaggerates the scale relationships, and the True-to-scale, a proportional model that more accurately represents the realistic scale relationships. Specifically, earlier exposure to the simplified model resulted in diminution of the conceptual gain from the subsequent realistic model, but the realistic model did not impede learning from the following simplified model.

[1]  G. Murphy,et al.  Feature Availability in Conceptual Combination , 1992 .

[2]  B. Latour,et al.  Human social origins: Oh please, tell us another story , 1986 .

[3]  Maria Kharitonova,et al.  The Role of Representations in Executive Function: Investigating a Developmental Link between Flexibility and Abstraction , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[4]  André Maurois,et al.  A Private Universe , 1932 .

[5]  A. Sfard On Two Metaphors for Learning and the Dangers of Choosing Just One , 1998 .

[6]  Gregory C. R. Yates,et al.  Visible Learning and the Science of How We Learn , 2013 .

[7]  F. Bartlett,et al.  Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology , 1932 .

[8]  Melvin S. Steinberg,et al.  Using models to teach electricity—The CASTLE project , 1993 .

[9]  R. Driver,et al.  Pupils and Paradigms: a Review of Literature Related to Concept Development in Adolescent Science Students , 1978 .

[10]  Mariana G. Hewson,et al.  The role of conceptual conflict in conceptual change and the design of science instruction , 1984 .

[11]  Nick Chater,et al.  WHY ARE CONJUNCTIVE CATEGORIES OVEREXTENDED , 1990 .

[12]  S. Reiss,et al.  Emotional disturbance and mental retardation: diagnostic overshadowing. , 1982, American journal of mental deficiency.

[13]  Shawn Y. Stevens,et al.  Developing a Hypothetical Multi-Dimensional Learning Progression for the Nature of Matter. , 2009 .

[14]  Dominic A Doran,et al.  Common student misconceptions in exercise physiology and biochemistry. , 2008, Advances in physiology education.

[15]  Yuko Munakata,et al.  Becoming self-directed: Abstract representations support endogenous flexibility in children , 2010, Cognition.

[16]  J. Singer,et al.  Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis , 2003 .

[17]  N. Orion,et al.  Cognitive Factors Affecting Student Understanding of Geologic Time. , 2003 .

[18]  Katherine L. McNeill,et al.  A learning progression for scientific argumentation: Understanding student work and designing supportive instructional contexts , 2010 .

[19]  Carolina Cruz-Neira,et al.  V-Volcano: Addressing Students' Misconceptions in Earth Sciences Learning through Virtual Reality Simulations , 2009, ISVC.

[20]  George E. P. Box,et al.  Empirical Model‐Building and Response Surfaces , 1988 .

[21]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Alignment in the Processing of Metaphor , 1997 .

[22]  N. M. Morris,et al.  On Looking into the Black Box: Prospects and Limits in the Search for Mental Models , 1986 .

[23]  Susan Carey,et al.  The effect of executive function on biological reasoning in young children: an individual differences study. , 2014, Child development.

[24]  R. Kozma The material features of multiple representations and their cognitive and social affordances for science understanding , 2003 .

[25]  R. Giere How Models Are Used to Represent Reality , 2004, Philosophy of Science.

[26]  Judy Perry,et al.  Evaluating two approaches to helping college students understand evolutionary trees through diagramming tasks. , 2008, CBE life sciences education.

[27]  Ronald N. Giere,et al.  Using Models to Represent Reality , 1999 .

[28]  Nancy J. Nersessian,et al.  Faraday to Einstein: Constructing Meaning in Scientific Theories , 1984 .

[29]  Guy H. Walker,et al.  A cognitive approach to threshold concepts , 2013 .

[30]  John K. Gilbert,et al.  Models and Modelling: Routes to More Authentic Science Education , 2004 .

[31]  S. Woolgar,et al.  Representation in Scientific Practice , 1990 .

[32]  C. A. Langford,et al.  Diagnostic and Treatment Overshadowing Bias Across Disabilities: Are Rehabilitation Professionals Immune? , 1994, Journal of Applied Rehabilitation Counseling.

[33]  William F. Brewer,et al.  The Nature and Functions of Schemas. Technical Report No. 325. , 1984 .

[34]  Ravit Golan Duncan,et al.  INFORMING A LEARNING PROGRESSION IN GENETICS: WHICH SHOULD BE TAUGHT FIRST, MENDELIAN INHERITANCE OR THE CENTRAL DOGMA OF MOLECULAR BIOLOGY? , 2016 .

[35]  John K. Gilbert,et al.  Models in explanations, Part 1 : Horses for courses? , 1998 .

[36]  Richard Lehrer,et al.  Seeding Evolutionary Thinking by Engaging Children in Modeling Its Foundations. , 2012 .

[37]  Aaron Rogat,et al.  Learning Progressions in Science: An Evidence-Based Approach to Reform. CPRE Research Report # RR-63. , 2009 .

[38]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Metaphors We Live By , 1980 .

[39]  Stacey Lowery Bretz,et al.  Generating cognitive dissonance in student interviews through multiple representations , 2012 .

[40]  Joel Michael,et al.  What makes physiology hard for students to learn? Results of a faculty survey. , 2007, Advances in physiology education.

[41]  Richard E. Mayer,et al.  Some Conditions of Meaningful Learning for Computer Programming: Advance Organizers and Subject Control of Frame Order. , 1976 .

[42]  Lorenzo Magnani,et al.  Model-Based Reasoning in Scientific Discovery , 1999, Springer US.

[43]  Philip M. Sadler,et al.  Conceptualizing astronomical scale: Virtual simulations on handheld tablet computers reverse misconceptions , 2014, Comput. Educ..

[44]  S. Reiss,et al.  Generality of diagnostic overshadowing across disciplines. , 1983, Applied research in mental retardation.

[45]  Charles W. Anderson,et al.  Developing a multi-year learning progression for carbon cycling in socio-ecological systems , 2009 .

[46]  Susan Carey,et al.  Science Education as Conceptual Change , 2000 .

[47]  David W. Goldsmith,et al.  The Great Clade Race: Presenting Cladistic Thinking to Biology Majors & General Science Students , 2003 .

[48]  Arthur Louis Odom,et al.  Action Potentials and Biology Textbooks: Accurate, Misconceptions or Avoidance?. , 1993 .

[49]  Ibrahim A. Halloun,et al.  The initial knowledge state of college physics students , 1985 .

[50]  María Pilar Jiménez Aleixandre Teaching evolution and natural selection: a look at textbooks and teachers , 2007 .

[51]  D. L. Hintzman,et al.  First impressions are lasting impressions: A primacy effect in memory for repetitions , 1997 .

[52]  Jennifer L. Snyder An investigation of the knowledge structures of experts, intermediates and novices in physics , 2000 .

[53]  D. Ausubel,et al.  Some psychological and educational limitations of learning by discovery , 2016 .

[54]  B. Reiser,et al.  Developing a learning progression for scientific modeling: Making scientific modeling accessible and meaningful for learners , 2009 .

[55]  B. Wilson,et al.  A review of cognitive teaching models , 1991 .

[56]  Ravit Golan Duncan,et al.  A Tale of Two Progressions: On the Benefits of Careful Comparisons , 2015 .

[57]  J. Keeley,et al.  The Commutative Property in Comorbid Diagnosis , 2013 .

[58]  Rand J. Spiro,et al.  Multiple analogies for complex concepts: antidotes for analogy-induced misconception in advanced knowledge acquisition , 1988 .

[59]  L. Boroditsky Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors , 2000, Cognition.

[60]  D. Gentner Structure‐Mapping: A Theoretical Framework for Analogy* , 1983 .

[61]  Martin Riopel,et al.  Differences in Brain Activation between Novices and Experts in Science during a Task Involving a Common Misconception in Electricity. , 2014 .

[62]  Jeremy K. Miller,et al.  Are first impressions lasting impressions? An exploration of the generality of the primacy effect in memory for repetitions , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[63]  H. Haste,et al.  Ambiguity, autonomy and agency: psychological challenges to new competence , 2001 .

[64]  Derek C. Briggs,et al.  Diagnostic Assessment With Ordered Multiple-Choice Items , 2006 .

[65]  Janet Vertesi,et al.  Representation in Scientific Practice Revisited , 2014 .

[66]  Jan H. F. Meyer,et al.  Overcoming Barriers to Student Understanding: Threshold Concepts and Troublesome Knowledge , 2006 .

[67]  Graham D. Low,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought: Metaphor and education , 2008 .

[68]  Robert Glaser,et al.  Model–based analysis and reasoning in science: The MARS curriculum , 1995 .

[69]  Robert McCartney,et al.  Threshold Concepts in Computer Science : a multinational empirical investigation , 2008 .

[70]  Mary Anne Rea-Ramirez,et al.  Determining Target Models and Effective Learning Pathways for Developing Understanding of Biological Topics , 2008 .

[71]  P. Hewson,et al.  Accommodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change , 1982 .

[72]  Z. Zacharia,et al.  Physical and Virtual Laboratories in Science and Engineering Education , 2013, Science.

[73]  Manjula D. Sharma,et al.  Raising cognitive load with linear multimedia to promote conceptual change , 2008 .

[74]  G. Norman,et al.  Development and Evaluation of the Conceptual Inventory of Natural Selection , 2002 .

[75]  Philip M. Sadler,et al.  The Astronomy and Space Science Concept Inventory: Development and Validation of Assessment Instruments Aligned with the K-12 National Science Standards , 2009 .

[76]  W. Leatherdale The role of analogy, model, and metaphor in science , 1974 .

[77]  L. Cameron Metaphor in Educational Discourse , 2003 .

[78]  Sam S Donovan,et al.  The Tree-Thinking Challenge , 2005, Science.

[79]  Ravit Golan Duncan,et al.  A Learning Progression for Deepening Students' Understandings of Modern Genetics across the 5th-10th Grades. , 2009 .

[80]  E. Boyes,et al.  Catastrophic misconceptions in science education , 1988 .

[81]  Brian W. Miller,et al.  Misconceptions of Astronomical Distances , 2010 .

[82]  Charles M. Reigeluth,et al.  The elaboration theory: Guidance for scope and sequence decisions. , 1999 .

[83]  John J. Clement Student/Teacher Co-construction of Visualizable Models in Large Group Discussion , 2008 .

[84]  Allan Collins,et al.  Cognitive Apprenticeship and Instructional Technology. Technical Report. , 1988 .

[85]  D. Gentner,et al.  Metaphor and knowledge change , 2000 .

[86]  Rosária Justi,et al.  The Application of a ‘Model of Modelling’ to Illustrate the Importance of Metavisualisation in Respect of the Three Types of Representation , 2009 .

[87]  Barbara Y. White,et al.  Dynamic mental models in learning science : The importance of constructing derivational linkages among models , 1999 .

[88]  William C. Philips Earth Science Misconceptions. , 1991 .

[89]  Eric Francoeur,et al.  The Forgotten Tool: The Design and Use of Molecular Models , 1997 .

[90]  R. Duschl,et al.  Learning progressions and teaching sequences: a review and analysis , 2011 .

[91]  Rowhea Elmesky,et al.  Building Capacity in Understanding Foundational Biology Concepts: A K-12 Learning Progression in Genetics Informed by Research on Children’s Thinking and Learning , 2013 .

[92]  John K. Gilbert,et al.  The use of analogue models by students of chemistry at higher education level , 1991 .

[93]  J. Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and Complex Learning: Recent Developments and Future Directions , 2005 .

[94]  Marianna Diomidous,et al.  The construction of a simulation-based system for the development of powerful and realistic models and practicals for healthcare professionals , 1998, IEEE Transactions on Information Technology in Biomedicine.

[95]  Joseph Krajcik,et al.  Building a learning progression for celestial motion: Elementary levels from an earth‐based perspective , 2010 .

[96]  E. Wisniewski Construal and Similarity in Conceptual Combination , 1996 .

[97]  Melvin S. Steinberg Target Model Sequence and Critical Learning Pathway for an Electricity Curriculum Based on Model Evolution , 2008 .

[98]  Christian Rogge,et al.  Misconceptions or Missing Conceptions , 2010 .

[99]  N. Wiener,et al.  The Role of Models in Science , 1945, Philosophy of Science.

[100]  Jan H. F. Meyer,et al.  Threshold concepts and troublesome knowledge (2): Epistemological considerations and a conceptual framework for teaching and learning , 2005 .

[101]  David W. Goldsmith,et al.  The Great Clade Race , 2009 .

[102]  Judy Perry,et al.  College Students' Misconceptions About Evolutionary Trees , 2007 .

[103]  R. Gagne Conditions of Learning , 1965 .

[104]  Nancy J. Nersessian,et al.  Model-Based Reasoning in Conceptual Change , 1999 .

[105]  David N. Ford,et al.  Effects of Time Scale Focus on System Understanding in Decision Support Systems , 2000 .