Graded Mirror Self-Recognition by Clark’s Nutcrackers

The traditional ‘mark test’ has shown some large-brained species are capable of mirror self-recognition. During this test a mark is inconspicuously placed on an animal’s body where it can only be seen with the aid of a mirror. If the animal increases the number of actions directed to the mark region when presented with a mirror, the animal is presumed to have recognized the mirror image as its reflection. However, the pass/fail nature of the mark test presupposes self-recognition exists in entirety or not at all. We developed a novel mirror-recognition task, to supplement the mark test, which revealed gradation in the self-recognition of Clark’s nutcrackers, a large-brained corvid. To do so, nutcrackers cached food alone, observed by another nutcracker, or with a regular or blurry mirror. The nutcrackers suppressed caching with a regular mirror, a behavioural response to prevent cache theft by conspecifics, but did not suppress caching with a blurry mirror. Likewise, during the mark test, most nutcrackers made more self-directed actions to the mark with a blurry mirror than a regular mirror. Both results suggest self-recognition was more readily achieved with the blurry mirror and that self-recognition may be more broadly present among animals than currently thought.

[1]  Joanna M. Dally,et al.  Cache protection strategies by western scrub-jays, Aphelocoma californica: implications for social cognition , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[2]  Manuel Soler,et al.  Mirror-Mark Tests Performed on Jackdaws Reveal Potential Methodological Problems in the Use of Stickers in Avian Mark-Test Studies , 2014, PloS one.

[3]  N. Clayton,et al.  Problems faced by food-caching corvids and the evolution of cognitive solutions , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[4]  D. Reiss,et al.  Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  S. Parker A developmental approach to the origins of self-recognition in great apes , 1991 .

[6]  D. Povinelli,et al.  Self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): distribution, ontogeny, and patterns of emergence , 1993 .

[7]  Thomas Suddendorf,et al.  The nature of visual self-recognition , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  P. Rochat Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[9]  Nicola S. Clayton,et al.  The Mentality of Crows: Convergent Evolution of Intelligence in Corvids and Apes , 2004, Science.

[10]  Self-recognition and abstraction abilities in the common chimpanzee studied with distorting mirrors. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  Thomas Bugnyar,et al.  Knower–guesser differentiation in ravens: others' viewpoints matter , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  N. Emery,et al.  Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays , 2001, Nature.

[13]  D. Povinelli,et al.  Self-recognition in young children using delayed versus live feedback: evidence of a developmental asynchrony. , 1996, Child development.

[14]  D. Kelly,et al.  Cache protection strategies of a non-social food-caching corvid, Clark’s nutcracker (Nucifragacolumbiana) , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[15]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Planning for the future by western scrub-jays , 2007, Nature.

[16]  T. Hothorn,et al.  Simultaneous Inference in General Parametric Models , 2008, Biometrical journal. Biometrische Zeitschrift.

[17]  R. Bos,et al.  A critical review of methodology and interpretation of mirror self-recognition research in nonhuman primates , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  G. Gallup Self-awareness and the evolution of social intelligence , 1998, Behavioural Processes.

[19]  Joanna M Dally,et al.  Food-Caching Western Scrub-Jays Keep Track of Who Was Watching When , 2006, Science.

[20]  O. Güntürkün,et al.  Mirror-Induced Behavior in the Magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of Self-Recognition , 2008, PLoS biology.

[21]  T. Bugnyar,et al.  Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: is it ‘tactical’ deception? , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  B. Skinner,et al.  "Self-awareness" in the pigeon. , 1981, Science.

[23]  G. Hunt Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows , 1996, Nature.

[24]  H. Bischof,et al.  Responses to mirror-image stimulation in jungle crows (Corvus macrorhynchos) , 2000, Animal Cognition.

[25]  D. Povinelli,et al.  Chimpanzees recognize themselves in mirrors , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  D. Povinelli,et al.  Further reflections on self-recognition in primates , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  A. H. Taylor,et al.  New Caledonian crows’ responses to mirrors , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[28]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Pilfering ravens, Corvus corax, adjust their behaviour to social context and identity of competitors , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[29]  S. Shettleworth The evolution of comparative cognition: Is the snark still a boojum? , 2009, Behavioural Processes.

[30]  Diana Reiss,et al.  Self-recognition in an Asian elephant , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  G. Gallup Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition , 1970, Science.

[32]  M. Bar Visual objects in context , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.