Beside the point: motor adaptation without feedback-based error correction in task-irrelevant conditions.

Adaptation of movement may be driven by the difference between planned and actual motor performance, or the difference between expected and actual sensory consequences of movement. To identify how the nervous system differentially uses these signals, we asked: does motor adaptation occur when movement errors are irrelevant to the task goal? Participants reached on a digitizing tablet from a fixed start location to one of three targets: a point, an arc, or a ray. For the arc, reaches could be in any direction, but to a specific extent. For the ray, reaches could be to any distance, but in a targeted direction. After baseline reaching to the point, the direction or extent of continuous visual feedback was perturbed during training with either a cursor rotation or gain, respectively, while reaching to either the ray (goal = direction) or the arc (goal = extent). The perturbation, therefore, was either relevant or irrelevant to the task goal, depending on target type. During interspersed catch trials, the perturbation was removed and the target switched back to the point, identical to baseline. Although the goal of baseline and catch trials was the same, significant aftereffects in catch trials indicated behavioral adaptation in response to the perturbation. Adaptation occurred regardless of whether the perturbation was relevant to the task, and it was independent of feedback control. The presence of adaptation orthogonal to task demands supports the hypothesis that the nervous system can rely on sensory prediction to drive motor learning that can generalize across tasks.

[1]  G. Fullerton Psychology and physiology. , 1896 .

[2]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[3]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  A. Kornheiser Adaptation to laterally displaced vision: a review. , 1976, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  L. Carlton Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  E. R. Crossman,et al.  Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[7]  R. N. Kackar,et al.  Approximations for Standard Errors of Estimators of Fixed and Random Effects in Mixed Linear Models , 1984 .

[8]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[9]  T. Milner,et al.  The effect of accuracy constraints on three-dimensional movement kinematics , 1990, Neuroscience.

[10]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance: evidence for direction-independent gain control , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  D. Elliott,et al.  Visual regulation of manual aiming , 1993 .

[12]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the regulation of visually guided aiming. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  Y Burnod,et al.  Learning a new visuomotor transformation: error correction and generalization. , 1995, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  M. Levin Interjoint coordination during pointing movements is disrupted in spastic hemiparesis. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Manual interception of moving targets II. On-line control of overlapping submovements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Saccadic gain modification: visual error drives motor adaptation. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Goal-Directed Aiming: Correcting a Force-Specification Error With the Right and Left Hands. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[22]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.

[24]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Michael A. Khan,et al.  The Effect of Practice on Component Submovements is Dependent on the Availability of Visual Feedback , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[26]  W. Helsen,et al.  A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[27]  B Wallace,et al.  Calibration and Alignment are Separable: Evidence From Prism Adaptation , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  F R Robinson,et al.  Visual error is the stimulus for saccade gain adaptation. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[29]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Perspectives and problems in motor learning , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  K. E. Novak,et al.  The use of overlapping submovements in the control of rapid hand movements , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Quantifying Generalization from Trial-by-Trial Behavior of Adaptive Systems that Learn with Basis Functions: Theory and Experiments in Human Motor Control , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational motor control , 2004 .

[34]  James Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  S. Scott Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  P. Morasso Spatial control of arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  J. Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  John M. Hollerbach,et al.  Dynamic interactions between limb segments during planar arm movement , 1982, Biological Cybernetics.

[39]  Gordon M. Redding,et al.  Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[40]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.

[41]  Benjamin Wallace,et al.  Generalization of prism adaptation. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Deric Wisleder,et al.  The role of different submovement types during pointing to a target , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  Paola Cesari,et al.  Body-goal Variability Mapping in an Aiming Task , 2006, Biological Cybernetics.

[45]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  Tim J. Anderson,et al.  Submovements in visually-guided and memory-guided reaching tasks: Changes in Parkinson's disease , 2008, 2008 30th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[47]  Sethu Vijayakumar,et al.  Unifying the Sensory and Motor Components of Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2008, NIPS.

[48]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Relevance of error: what drives motor adaptation? , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  John W Krakauer,et al.  Motor learning and consolidation: the case of visuomotor rotation. , 2009, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[50]  Stephan Riek,et al.  Real-time error detection but not error correction drives automatic visuomotor adaptation , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  Hiroaki Gomi,et al.  Multiple Motor Learning Strategies in Visuomotor Rotation , 2010, PloS one.

[52]  Erin K Cressman,et al.  Reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration following exposure to misaligned sensory input. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[54]  Joaquin A. Anguera,et al.  A spatial explicit strategy reduces error but interferes with sensorimotor adaptation. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  J. Banquet,et al.  Parkinson's disease differentially affects adaptation to gradual as compared to sudden visuomotor distortions. , 2011, Human movement science.