An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Orienting of Attention

Attention can be voluntarily directed to a location or automatically summoned to a location by a salient stimulus. We compared the effects of voluntary and stimulus-driven shifts of spatial attention on the blood oxygenation level-dependent signal in humans, using a method that separated preparatory activity related to the initial shift of attention from the subsequent activity caused by target presentation. Voluntary shifts produced greater preparatory activity than stimulus-driven shifts in the frontal eye field (FEF) and intraparietal sulcus, core regions of the dorsal frontoparietal attention network, demonstrating their special role in the voluntary control of attention. Stimulus-driven attentional shifts to salient color singletons recruited occipitotemporal regions, sensitive to color information and part of the dorsal network, including the FEF, suggesting a partly overlapping circuit for endogenous and exogenous orienting. The right temporoparietal junction (TPJ), a core region of the ventral frontoparietal attention network, was strongly modulated by stimulus-driven attentional shifts to behaviorally relevant stimuli, such as targets at unattended locations. However, the TPJ did not respond to salient, task-irrelevant color singletons, indicating that behavioral relevance is critical for TPJ modulation during stimulus-driven orienting. Finally, both ventral and dorsal regions were modulated during reorienting but significantly only by reorienting after voluntary shifts, suggesting the importance of a mismatch between expectation and sensory input.

[1]  J. Jonides Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .

[2]  R. Desimone,et al.  Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[4]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[5]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention , 1989, Vision Research.

[6]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  P T Fox,et al.  A Highly Accurate Method of Localizing Regions of Neuronal Activation in the Human Brain with Positron Emission Tomography , 1989, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[8]  H. J. Muller,et al.  Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[12]  R. Abrams,et al.  Inhibition of return: effects of attentional cuing on eye movement latencies. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[14]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[15]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[18]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[19]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8 , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Attentional control during visual search: the effect of irrelevant singletons. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  Neural Basis of Endogenous and Exogenous Spatial Orienting: A Functional MRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[23]  A. Nobre,et al.  The Large-Scale Neural Network for Spatial Attention Displays Multifunctional Overlap But Differential Asymmetry , 1999, NeuroImage.

[24]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Areas Involved in Encoding and Applying Directional Expectations to Moving Objects , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Jonathan E. Jennings,et al.  An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-Hue test reveals multiple color-selective areas in human ventral occipitotemporal cortex. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  R. Goebel,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of target detection: an fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Neuronal coding of prediction errors. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[28]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Downar,et al.  A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Attention: Object-Based Selection of a Region in Space , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[32]  W. Schultz,et al.  Modifications of reward expectation-related neuronal activity during learning in primate orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  S. Zeki,et al.  The architecture of the colour centre in the human visual brain: new results and a review * , 2000, The European journal of neuroscience.

[34]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Visual attention and cortical circuits , 2001 .

[35]  M. A. Steinmetz,et al.  Neuronal responses in area 7a to multiple-stimulus displays: I. neurons encode the location of the salient stimulus. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Separating Processes within a Trial in Event-Related Functional MRI II. Analysis , 2001, NeuroImage.

[37]  M. Sereno,et al.  Mapping of Contralateral Space in Retinotopic Coordinates by a Parietal Cortical Area in Humans , 2001, Science.

[38]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Neural sources involved in auditory target detection and novelty processing: an event-related fMRI study. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[39]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Separating Processes within a Trial in Event-Related Functional MRI I. The Method , 2001, NeuroImage.

[40]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  S. Pollmann,et al.  Covert Reorienting and Inhibition of Return: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Supramodal Effects of Covert Spatial Orienting Triggered by Visual or Tactile Events , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[44]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Human fMRI evidence for the neural correlates of preparatory set , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Windows on the brain: the emerging role of atlases and databases in neuroscience , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[46]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Reactivation of networks involved in preparatory states. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Neural Systems for Visual Orienting and Their Relationships to Spatial Working Memory , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  Tony Ro,et al.  Inhibition of return and the human frontal eye fields , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Functional Organization of Human Intraparietal and Frontal Cortex for Attending, Looking, and Pointing , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Quantitative analysis of attention and detection signals during visual search. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the same large-scale neural network , 2004, NeuroImage.

[53]  W. Newsome,et al.  Matching Behavior and the Representation of Value in the Parietal Cortex , 2004, Science.

[54]  Andrew R. Mayer,et al.  An Event-related fMRI Study of Exogenous Orienting: Supporting Evidence for the Cortical Basis of Inhibition of Return? , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Coordination of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Attentional Control in Human Cortex , 2005, Psychological science.

[56]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  From Attention to Action in Frontal Cortex , 2006 .

[57]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Visuospatial reorienting signals in the human temporo‐parietal junction are independent of response selection , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.