Prefrontal hyperactivity in older people during motor planning

The aim of this study was to assess the influence of age-related changes in cortical activity related to the motor preparation involved in simple- and discriminative-reaction tasks. To distinguish between age effects on motor planning and stimulus processing, both movement- and stimulus-locked event related potentials (ERPs) were investigated in 14 younger, 14 middle-aged, and 14 older adults (mean ages 24.4, 49, and 70 years, respectively). The novel results of the present study are the prefrontal over-recruitment observed in older adults in movement-related cortical potentials (MRCPs) and the differential pattern of aging effects observed at behavioral and at electrophysiological level between middle-aged and older adults. Overall, the following results were observed: (i) behavioral results confirmed the well-known slowing of responses in aging people, which were associated with optimal accuracy; (ii) the age-related differences in cortical activity underlying the generation of voluntary movements in response to external stimuli were more pronounced for the motor planning than the stimulus processing stage; (iii) the source and the time-course analysis of the over-recruitment in the older adults indicated tonic involvement of prefrontal areas regardless of task complexity; and (iv) middle-aged adults showed a 'young adult-like' behavioral speed, but an 'older adult-like' overactivation of prefrontal areas. In summary, to reach the same accuracy, older subjects prepared the action with greater anticipation and higher cost, as indexed by the earlier latency onset and larger prefrontal cortical activation.

[1]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Lack of age effects on human brain potentials preceding voluntary movements , 1990, Neuroscience Letters.

[2]  P. Derambure,et al.  Influence of aging on cortical activity associated with a visuo-motor task , 2004, Neurobiology of Aging.

[3]  C. Barnes,et al.  Neural plasticity in the ageing brain , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[4]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  Overrecruitment in the Aging Brain as a Function of Task Demands: Evidence for a Compensatory View , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Effects of aging on slowing of motor-response generation. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[6]  S. Rossi,et al.  Clinical neurophysiology of aging brain: From normal aging to neurodegeneration , 2007, Progress in Neurobiology.

[7]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action , 1986 .

[8]  M. Hallett,et al.  What is the Bereitschaftspotential? , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[9]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Systems Neuroplasticity in the Aging Brain: Recruiting Additional Neural Resources for Successful Motor Performance in Elderly Persons , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Peter S. Jones,et al.  Does healthy aging affect the hemispheric activation balance during paced index-to-thumb opposition task? An fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.

[11]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  N. Ward Compensatory mechanisms in the aging motor system , 2006, Ageing Research Reviews.

[13]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Neural correlates of fast stimulus discrimination and response selection in top-level fencers , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[14]  Joseph T. Gwin,et al.  Motor control and aging: Links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[15]  T. Miles,et al.  Age and sex differences in human motor cortex input–output characteristics , 2003, The Journal of physiology.

[16]  M. M. C. Berg-Lenssen,et al.  A spatiotemporal dipole model of the stimulus preceding negativity (spn) prior to feedback stimuli , 2005, Brain Topography.

[17]  P. Reuter-Lorenz New visions of the aging mind and brain , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Sequence effects support the conflict theory of N2 and P3 in the Go/NoGo task. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[19]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Benefits of sports participation for executive function in disabled athletes. , 2010, Journal of neurotrauma.

[20]  Juliana Yordanova,et al.  Motor-response generation as a source of aging-related behavioural slowing in choice-reaction tasks , 2006, Neurobiology of Aging.

[21]  R. Davidson,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory IV , 1976 .

[22]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Effects of ageing on cognitive task preparation as reflected by event-related potentials , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  D. Stuss,et al.  Age-related differences in processing irrelevant information: Evidence from event-related potentials , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  Targets and non-targets in the aging brain: A go/nogo event-related potential study , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[26]  N. Cohen,et al.  General and task-specific frontal lobe recruitment in older adults during executive processes: A fMRI investigation of task-switching , 2001, Neuroreport.

[27]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Sport is not always healthy: Executive brain dysfunction in professional boxers. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[28]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  Multichannel auditory event-related brain potentials: effects of normal aging on the scalp distribution of N1, P2, N2 and P300 latencies and amplitudes. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[29]  T. Salthouse Aging and measures of processing speed , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[30]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Effects of aging on event-related brain potentials and reaction times in an auditory oddball task. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[31]  H. Siebner,et al.  Age-related decrease in paired-pulse intracortical inhibition in the human primary motor cortex , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[32]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Sensorimotor slowing with ageing is mediated by a functional dysregulation of motor-generation processes: evidence from high-resolution event-related potentials. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  H Ishizuka,et al.  [Age-related changes in movement-related cortical potentials]. , 1996, Nihon Ronen Igakkai zasshi. Japanese journal of geriatrics.

[34]  Annette Sterr,et al.  Neural correlates of movement preparation in healthy ageing , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[35]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[36]  H. Shibasaki,et al.  Cortical potentials preceding voluntary movement: evidence for three periods of preparation in man. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  Edward J. Golob,et al.  Event-related potentials accompanying motor preparation and stimulus expectancy in the young, young-old and oldest-old , 2005, Neurobiology of Aging.

[38]  Denise C. Park,et al.  The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[39]  Alexa B. Roggeveen,et al.  Lateralized readiness potentials reveal motor slowing in the aging brain. , 2007, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.