The neural correlates of conscious experience: an experimental framework

Demonstrating that neural activity 'represents' physical properties of the world such as the orientation of a line in the receptive field of a nerve cell is a standard procedure in neuroscience. However, not all such neural activity will be associated with the mental representations that form the contents of consciousness. In some cases, such as when patients with blindsight correctly 'guess' the location of a stimulus, neural activity is associated with physical stimulation and with appropriate behaviour, but not with awareness. To identify the neural correlates of conscious experience we need to identify patterns of neural activity that are specifically associated with awareness. Experiments aimed at making such identifications require that subjects report some aspect of their conscious experience either verbally or through some pre-arranged non-verbal report while neural activity is measured. If there is some characteristic neural signature of consciousness, then this should be distinguishable from the kinds of neural activity associated with stimulation and/or behaviour in the absence of awareness. It remains to be seen whether the neural signature of consciousness relates to the location of the neural activity, the temporal properties of the neural activity or the form of the interaction between activity in different brain regions.

[1]  F. Lhermitte 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  F. Crick The Astonishing Hypothesis , 1994 .

[3]  J. Hornak,et al.  Visual neglect in the monkey. Representation and disconnection. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Temporal dissociation of motor responses and subjective awareness. A study in normal subjects. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  R. Clark,et al.  Classical conditioning and brain systems: the role of awareness. , 1998, Science.

[6]  H. Noice,et al.  Two approaches to learning a theatrical script. , 1996, Memory.

[7]  R. Bauer,et al.  Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: A neuropsychological application of the guilty knowledge test , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  G. Edelman,et al.  Neural dynamics in a model of the thalamocortical system. II. The role of neural synchrony tested through perturbations of spike timing. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  A. Milner,et al.  Neuropsychology of Consciousness , 2019, Frontiers Research Topics.

[10]  B. Libet,et al.  The neural time factor in conscious and unconscious events. , 2007, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[11]  D. Brooks,et al.  A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  A. Aertsen,et al.  Neuronal assemblies , 1989, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[13]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  On the relation between memory and consciousness: Dissociable interactions and conscious experience. , 1989 .

[14]  R. Llinás,et al.  Of dreaming and wakefulness , 1991, Neuroscience.

[15]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Parietal neglect and visual awareness , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Three hands: fragmentation of human bodily awareness , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[17]  L. Weiskrantz Blindsight : a case study and implications , 1986 .

[18]  M. Kinsbourne Integrated field theory of consciousness , 1992 .

[19]  Nicholas D. Schiff,et al.  The Thalamic Intralaminar Nuclei: A Role in Visual Awareness , 1997 .

[20]  M. Jüptner,et al.  Review: Does Measurement of Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Reflect Synaptic Activity?—Implications for PET and fMRI , 1995, NeuroImage.

[21]  M. Jouvet What does a cat dream about? , 1979, Trends in Neurosciences.

[22]  S. Zeki A vision of the brain , 1993 .

[23]  R. Dolan,et al.  Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala , 1998, Nature.

[24]  E. Heit,et al.  Similarity and property effects in inductive reasoning. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  D. Schacter,et al.  Access to consciousness: Dissociations between implicit and explicit knowledge , 1986 .

[26]  A. Cowey,et al.  Blindsight in monkeys , 1995, Nature.

[27]  C. Koch,et al.  Consciousness and neuroscience. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Spatiotemporal Patterns of Spindle Oscillations in Cortex and Thalamus , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[30]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys' percepts during binocular rivalry , 1996, Nature.

[31]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Sleep: off-line memory reprocessing , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  J. Jastrow,et al.  On small differences in sensation , 1884 .

[33]  J. Easterbrook The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. , 1959, Psychological review.

[34]  S Zeki,et al.  Conscious visual perception without V1. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[35]  E Donchin,et al.  The truth will out: interrogative polygraphy ("lie detection") with event-related brain potentials. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[36]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Memory following Surgical Anesthesia , 1990 .

[37]  E. Wisniewski When concepts combine , 1997, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  A. McIntosh,et al.  Structural modeling of functional neural pathways mapped with 2-deoxyglucose: effects of acoustic startle habituation on the auditory system , 1991, Brain Research.

[39]  R. Passingham,et al.  Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness , 1996, Nature.

[41]  R. Bartus,et al.  The Calpain Hypothesis of Neurodegeneration: Evidence for a Common Cytotoxic Pathway , 1997 .

[42]  G. Rees,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. , 1998, Science.

[43]  C. Koch,et al.  Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? , 1995, Nature.

[44]  J. Hobson The Dreaming Brain , 1988 .

[45]  C D Frith,et al.  Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. , 1997, Science.

[46]  A. Ohman,et al.  "Unconscious anxiety": phobic responses to masked stimuli. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[47]  A. Milner Streams and consciousness: visual awareness and the brain , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  Seth J. Ramus,et al.  Intact Artificial Grammar Learning in Amnesia: Dissociation of Classification Learning and Explicit Memory for Specific Instances , 1992 .

[49]  A. Mayes,et al.  Autonomic and Recognition Indices of Memory in Amnesic and Healthy Control Subjects , 1996, Cortex.

[50]  J. Hobson Dreaming as delirium: a mental status analysis of our nightly madness. , 1996, Seminars in neurology.

[51]  Hobson Ja Dreaming as Delirium: A Mental Status Analysis of our Nightly Madness , 1997, Seminars in neurology.

[52]  D. Schacter Consciousness and Awareness in Memory and Amnesia: Critical Issues , 1992 .

[53]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Neuroanatomical Correlates of Veridical and Illusory Recognition Memory: Evidence from Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, Neuron.

[54]  R L Buckner,et al.  Functional neuroimaging studies of encoding, priming, and explicit memory retrieval. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[55]  E. Wisniewski Construal and Similarity in Conceptual Combination , 1996 .

[56]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Implicit Short-Lived Motor Representations of Space in Brain Damaged and Healthy Subjects , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.

[57]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.

[58]  W. Singer,et al.  Reduced Synchronization in the Visual Cortex of Cats with Strabismic Amblyopia , 1994, The European journal of neuroscience.

[59]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  A functional neuroanatomy of hallucinations in schizophrenia , 1995, Nature.

[60]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding. A PET study. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[61]  U E Gallo,et al.  Locked-in syndrome: report of a case. , 1989, The American journal of emergency medicine.

[62]  A. Iwanami,et al.  Event‐related potential correlates of functional hearing loss: Reduced P3 amplitude with preserved Nl and N2 components in a unilateral case , 1996, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[63]  N. Lavie Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[64]  P W Burgess,et al.  Confabulation and the control of recollection. , 1996, Memory.

[65]  Clelia Marchetti Sergio Della Sala Disentangling the Alien and Anarchic Hand , 1998 .

[66]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory , 1998, Nature.

[67]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. , 1980, Science.