Cognitive Control Functions of Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Macaque Monkeys Performing a Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Analog

Monkeys were trained to select one of three targets by matching in color or matching in shape to a sample. Because the matching rule frequently changed and there were no cues for the currently relevant rule, monkeys had to maintain the relevant rule in working memory to select the correct target. We found that monkeys' error commission was not limited to the period after the rule change and occasionally occurred even after several consecutive correct trials, indicating that the task was cognitively demanding. In trials immediately after such error trials, monkeys' speed of selecting targets was slower. Additionally, in trials following consecutive correct trials, the monkeys' target selections for erroneous responses were slower than those for correct responses. We further found evidence for the involvement of the cortex in the anterior cingulate sulcus (ACCs) in these error-related behavioral modulations. First, ACCs cell activity differed between after-error and after-correct trials. In another group of ACCs cells, the activity differed depending on whether the monkeys were making a correct or erroneous decision in target selection. Second, bilateral ACCs lesions significantly abolished the response slowing both in after-error trials and in error trials. The error likelihood in after-error trials could be inferred by the error feedback in the previous trial, whereas the likelihood of erroneous responses after consecutive correct trials could be monitored only internally. These results suggest that ACCs represent both context-dependent and internally detected error likelihoods and promote modes of response selections in situations that involve these two types of error likelihood.

[1]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[3]  B. Richmond,et al.  Anterior Cingulate: Single Neuronal Signals Related to Degree of Reward Expectancy , 2002, Science.

[4]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Behavioral evidence for working memory of sensory dimension in macaque monkeys , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[5]  T. Robbins,et al.  The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[6]  M. Raichle,et al.  The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  P. Dayan,et al.  Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans , 2006, Nature.

[8]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Cortical Signals for Rewarded Actions and Strategic Exploration , 2013, Neuron.

[9]  D. Tucker,et al.  Electrophysiological Responses to Errors and Feedback in the Process of Action Regulation , 2003, Psychological science.

[10]  A. Turken,et al.  Dissociation between conflict detection and error monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  E. Procyk,et al.  Behavioral Shifts and Action Valuation in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2008, Neuron.

[12]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  Bryan Rodgers,et al.  What does a Man do after he Makes an Error? An Analysis of Response Programming , 1977 .

[14]  T. Shallice,et al.  The origins of utilization behaviour. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Foraging , 2012, Science.

[16]  G. Smith,et al.  How normal and retarded individuals monitor and regulate speed and accuracy of responding in serial choice tasks. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[17]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Switching from automatic to controlled action by monkey medial frontal cortex , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Learning the value of information in an uncertain world , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Medial prefrontal cell activity signaling prediction errors of action values , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[22]  H. Seo,et al.  Temporal Filtering of Reward Signals in the Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex during a Mixed-Strategy Game , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  J. Tanji,et al.  Role for cingulate motor area cells in voluntary movement selection based on reward. , 1998, Science.

[24]  E. Procyk,et al.  Anterior cingulate activity during routine and non-routine sequential behaviors in macaques , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[26]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Conflict-induced behavioural adjustment: a clue to the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  T. Paus Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and cognition interface , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[28]  V. B. Brooks,et al.  ‘Error’ potentials in limbic cortex (anterior cingulate area 24) of monkeys during motor learning , 1986, Neuroscience Letters.

[29]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of Goal-Based Motor Selection in the Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[30]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[31]  Richard Passingham,et al.  Delayed matching after selective prefrontal lesions in monkeys (Macaca mulatta) , 1975, Brain Research.

[32]  D Laming,et al.  Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[33]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Learned Predictions of Error Likelihood in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Science.

[34]  Philip G. F. Browning,et al.  Dissociable Components of Rule-Guided Behavior Depend on Distinct Medial and Prefrontal Regions , 2009, Science.

[35]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[36]  R. Simons,et al.  To err is autonomic: error-related brain potentials, ANS activity, and post-error compensatory behavior. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[37]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Mnemonic Function of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Conflict-Induced Behavioral Adjustment , 2007, Science.

[38]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Prefrontal Cell Activities Related to Monkeys' Success and Failure in Adapting to Rule Changes in a Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Analog , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  David Goodman,et al.  Performance Monitoring in the Anterior Cingulate is Not All Error Related: Expectancy Deviation and the Representation of Action-Outcome Associations , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  R. Passingham,et al.  Non-reversal shifts after selective prefrontal ablations in monkeys (Macaca mulatta). , 1972, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  John M. Pearson,et al.  Neuronal basis of sequential foraging decisions in a patchy environment , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  L. Fellows,et al.  Dorsal Medial Prefrontal Cortex Plays a Necessary Role in Rapid Error Prediction in Humans , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.