Contextual control of equivalence-based transformation of functions.
暂无分享,去创建一个
David E. Greenway | M. Dougher | Michael J. Dougher | David R. Perkins | Ashton Koons | Carmenne Chiasson | D. Greenway | D. R. Perkins | Ashton N. Koons | Carmenne Chiasson
[1] David E. Greenway,et al. The transfer of respondent eliciting and extinction functions through stimulus equivalence classes. , 1994, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[2] R. Stromer,et al. Complex Stimulus Control and Equivalence , 1993 .
[3] S. Hayes,et al. Some Applied Implications of a Contemporary Behavior-Analytic Account of Verbal Events , 1993, The Behavior analyst.
[4] K. Lashley. Conditional Reactions in the Rat , 1938 .
[5] B. Roche,et al. Relational frame theory : A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition , 2004 .
[6] B. Roche,et al. A Transfer of Functions and a Conditional Transfer of Functions Through Equivalence Relations in Three-To Six-Year-Old Children , 1995 .
[7] S. Hayes,et al. The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence relations. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[8] Barbara S. Kohlenberg,et al. The transfer of contextual control over equivalence classes through equivalence classes: a possible model of social stereotyping. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[9] R R Saunders,et al. The nonequivalence of behavioral and mathematical equivalence. , 1992, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[10] S. Hayes,et al. Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[11] Linda J. Hayes,et al. Conceptual Differences in the Analysis of Stimulus Equivalence , 1999 .
[12] R. Lazar,et al. Second-order control of sequence-class equivalences in children , 1986, Behavioural Processes.
[13] D. Barnes,et al. A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness, more than, and less than. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[14] D. Crowne,et al. Experimental foundations of clinical psychology , 1964 .
[15] M. Sidman,et al. Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency. , 2000, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[16] D. Lawrence. The nature of a stimulus: Some relationships between learning and perception. , 1962 .
[17] R. Lazar. Extending sequence-class membership with matching to sample. , 1977, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[18] B. Roche,et al. Contextual Control over the Derived Transformation of Discriminative and Sexual Arousal Functions , 2000 .
[19] M. Sidman,et al. Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: an expansion of the testing paradigm. , 1982, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[20] M. Dougher,et al. A Behavior-Analytic Account of Depression and a Case Report Using Acceptance-Based Procedures , 2003, The Behavior analyst.
[21] E. F. Meehan,et al. Contextual Control Of New Equivalence Classes , 1995 .
[22] C. Osgood. Method and theory in experimental psychology , 1953 .
[23] J. D. de Rose,et al. Emergent simple discrimination established by indirect relation to differential consequences. , 1988, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[24] J. Spradlin,et al. The development of stimulus classes using match-to-sample procedures: Sample classification versus comparison classification☆ , 1986 .
[25] D. Barnes,et al. Analyzing derived stimulus relations requires more than the concept of stimulus class. , 1997, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[26] Bryan Roche,et al. Relational Frame Theory and Stimulus Equivalence are Fundamentally Different: A Reply to Saunders’ Commentary , 1996 .
[27] S. Hayes,et al. Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. , 1988, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[28] D. Barnes,et al. Stimulus equivalence and relational frame theory , 1994 .
[29] Transfer of Consequential Functions VIA Stimulus Equivalence: Generalization to Different Testing Contexts , 1996 .
[30] S. Hayes,et al. Criticisms of relational frame theory: Implications for a behavior analytic account of derived stimulus relations , 1996 .
[31] S. Hayes,et al. 14 Stimulus classes and stimulus relations: Arbitrarily applicable relational responding as an operant , 1996 .
[32] Henry C. Ellis,et al. Transfer of Learning , 2021, Research in Mathematics Education.
[33] R R Saunders,et al. The role of instructions in the transfer of ordinal functions through equivalence classes. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[34] M Sidman,et al. Functional classes and equivalence relations. , 1989, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[35] G. Green,et al. Development and crossmodal transfer of contextual control of emergent stimulus relations. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[36] S. Hayes,et al. Why it is crucial to understand thinking and feeling: An analysis and application to drug abuse , 2000, The Behavior analyst.
[37] B. Roche,et al. A transformation of respondently conditioned stimulus function in accordance with arbitrarily applicable relations. , 1997, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[38] C. Kennedy,et al. Second-Order Conditional Control of Symmetric and Transitive Stimulus Relations: The Influence of Order Effects , 1988 .
[39] Transfer of contextual stimulus function via equivalence class development. , 1989, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[40] M Sidman,et al. Contextual control of emergent equivalence relations. , 1989, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[41] Dermot Barnes-Holmes,et al. Explaining Complex Behavior: Two Perspectives on the Concept Of Generalized Operant Classes , 2000 .
[42] S. Hayes,et al. Verbal relations and the evolution of behavior analysis. , 1992 .
[43] M. Markham,et al. 8 Stimulus classes and the untrained acquisition of stimulus functions , 1996 .