Differential visual processing for equivalent retinal information from near versus far space

Converging evidence from animal neurophysiology and human clinical studies has suggested that visual information arising from near versus far space may be mediated predominantly by different visual subsystems in the human brain. In five experiments, healthy observers either detected or identified brief peripheral targets presented in near (peripersonal) versus far (extrapersonal) space. Apparent size (subtended visual angle) and luminance were equated to provide equivalent retinal information across near and far viewing conditions. Peripheral detection accuracy declined more rapidly with increasing target eccentricity in far viewing versus near viewing conditions. Peripheral identification accuracy under similar conditions showed no such dissociation of near versus far processing with eccentricity. These data suggest that retinal information from near versus far space may be preferentially processed by substantially different neural substrates, with active modulation of the relative contributions of involved magnocellular-dorsal and parvocellular-ventral visual pathways, depending on various potential ecological uses of the retinal information.

[1]  Glyn W Humphreys,et al.  Widening the Sphere of Influence: Using a Tool to Extend Extrapersonal Visual Space in a Patient with Severe Neglect , 2002, Neurocase.

[2]  John M. Allman,et al.  The Effect of Gaze Angle and Fixation Distance on the Responses of Neurons in V1, V2, and V4 , 2002, Neuron.

[3]  Paul J. Eslinger,et al.  Perceptual–attentional and motor-intentional bias in near and far space , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[4]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Deficits in attention and movement following the removal of postarcuate (area 6) and prearcuate (area 8) cortex in macaque monkeys. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[6]  W. Epstein,et al.  Perception of space and motion , 1995 .

[7]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  On the nature of near space: Effects of tool use and the transition to far space , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Mark E. McCourt,et al.  Asymmetries of Visuospatial Attention are Modulated by Viewing Distance and Visual Field Elevation: Pseudoneglect in Peripersonal and Extrapersonal Space , 2000, Cortex.

[9]  Beverly C. Butler,et al.  Gradients of detection in neglect: comparison of peripersonal and extrapersonal space , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  S. Wu,et al.  Adler's Physiology of the Eye , 2002 .

[12]  A. Berti,et al.  When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  O. Grüsser,et al.  Multimodal Structure of the Extrapersonal Space , 1983 .

[14]  A. Dobbins,et al.  Distance modulation of neural activity in the visual cortex. , 1998, Science.

[15]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[16]  James E. Cutting,et al.  Chapter 3 – Perceiving Layout and Knowing Distances: The Integration, Relative Potency, and Contextual Use of Different Information about Depth* , 1995 .

[17]  T. Kuhlen,et al.  Horizontal and vertical pseudoneglect in peri- and extrapersonal space , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[18]  A. Cowey,et al.  Left visuo-spatial neglect can be worse in far than in near space , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  J. Pettigrew,et al.  Parallel processing of binocular disparity in the cat’s retinogeniculocortical pathways , 1987, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[20]  T. Schenk,et al.  An allocentric rather than perceptual deficit in patient D.F. , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  J. Marshall,et al.  Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.

[22]  K. Zilles,et al.  Neural consequences of acting in near versus far space: a physiological basis for clinical dissociations. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  Paul Azzopardi,et al.  Uneven mapping of magnocellular and parvocellular projections from the lateral geniculate nucleus to the striate cortex in the macaque monkey , 1999, Vision Research.

[24]  D. Dacey,et al.  Dendritic field size and morphology of midget and parasol ganglion cells of the human retina. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  J. Beaumont,et al.  Line bisection in normal adults: direction of attentional bias for near and far space , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  J. Malpeli,et al.  Laminar and retinotopic organization of the macaque lateral geniculate nucleus: Magnocellular and parvocellular magnification functions , 1996, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[27]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[28]  T Landis,et al.  Near and far visual space in unilateral neglect , 1998, Annals of neurology.

[29]  K. Heilman,et al.  Neglect and Related Disorders , 1984, Seminars in neurology.

[30]  Gereon R Fink,et al.  Are action and perception in near and far space additive or interactive factors? , 2003, NeuroImage.

[31]  F. Previc Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[32]  I. Keller,et al.  Visuospatial neglect in near and far space: dissociation between line bisection and letter cancellation , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  A. Cowey,et al.  No abrupt change in visual hemineglect from near to far space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[35]  R. McIntosh,et al.  Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action? , 2010, Cognitive neuroscience.

[36]  Stefano Cappa,et al.  Visual Neglect for Far and Near Extra-Personal Space in Humans , 1989, Cortex.

[37]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Spatially Oriented Behavior , 2011 .

[38]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Book Review: Gain Modulation in the Central Nervous System: Where Behavior, Neurophysiology, and Computation Meet , 2001, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[39]  K. Heilman,et al.  Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task , 1980, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Fred H. Previc,et al.  The Neuropsychology of 3-D Space , 1998 .