Relationship of Preference Judgments to Typicality, Novelty, and Mere Exposure

Two theories of aesthetic preference are reviewed. According to Berlyne's psychobiological theory, the main determinants of aesthetic preference are collative variables such as complexity and novelty, and preference is generally related to its determinants in an inverted-U manner. Recent findings contrary to these predictions are briefly reviewed. According to Martindale's cognitive theory, the main determinant of aesthetic preference is prototypicality or meaningfulness, and preference is usually related to its determinants in a monotonic or U-shaped fashion. Three experiments concerning preference for semantic category exemplars are reported. Typicality accounted for eight or nine times more of explained variance in preference than did long-term novelty, short-term novelty, or mere exposure. Preference was related to both typicality and collative variables by monotonic or U-shaped functions.

[1]  J. Guilford The affective value of color as a function of hue, tint, and chroma , 1934 .

[2]  J. P. Guilford,et al.  There is system in color preferences , 1940 .

[3]  System in the relationship of affective value to frequency and intensity of auditory stimuli. , 1954 .

[4]  System in the relationship of affective value to frequency and intensity of auditory stimuli. , 1954, The American journal of psychology.

[5]  G. W. Granger An Experimental Study of Colour Preferences , 1955 .

[6]  R. Plomp,et al.  The connotation of musical consonance , 1962 .

[7]  Harry Munsinger,et al.  Uncertainty, structure, and preference. , 1964 .

[8]  D. Berlyne,et al.  Effects of auditory pitch and complexity on EEG desynchronization and on verbally expressed judgments. , 1967, Canadian journal of psychology.

[9]  H. Day Evaluations of subjective complexity, pleasingness and interestingness for a series of random polygons varying in complexity , 1967 .

[10]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[11]  R. Zajonc Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. , 1968 .

[12]  H. Day,et al.  The importance of symmetry and complexity in the evaluation of complexity, interest and pleasingness , 1968 .

[13]  W. Montague,et al.  Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms , 1969 .

[14]  P. Vitz,et al.  Preference for tones as a function of frequency (hertz) and intensity (decibels) , 1972 .

[15]  David J. Stang,et al.  Methodological factors in mere exposure research. , 1974 .

[16]  E. Rosch The nature of mental codes for color categories. , 1975 .

[17]  R. Herrnstein,et al.  Toward a law of response strength. , 1976 .

[18]  T. Whitfield,et al.  The effects of categorization and prototypicality on aesthetic choice in a furniture selection task. , 1979 .

[19]  R. Zajonc Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .

[20]  Leah L. Light,et al.  Why Attractive People are Harder to Remember , 1981 .

[21]  The Aesthetic Experience and Mundane Reality , 1984 .

[22]  Colin Martindale,et al.  The pleasures of thought: A theory of cognitive hedonics. , 1984 .

[23]  C. Martindale The Evolution of Italian Painting: A Quantitative Investigation of Trends in Style and Content from the Late Gothic to the Rococo Period , 2017 .

[24]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[25]  C. Martindale,et al.  Priming, prototypicality, and preference. , 1988 .