Electrophysiological correlates of affective blindsight

An EEG investigation was carried out in a patient with complete cortical blindness who presented affective blindsight, i.e. who performed above chance when asked to guess the emotional expressions on a series of faces. To uncover the electrophysiological mechanisms involved in this phenomenon we combined multivariate pattern recognition (MPR) with local field potential estimates provided by electric source imaging (ELECTRA). All faces, including neutral faces, elicited distinctive oscillatory EEG patterns that were correctly identified by the MPR algorithm as belonging to the class of facial expressions actually presented. Consequently, neural responses in this patient are not restricted to emotionally laden faces. Earliest non-specific differences between faces occur from 70 ms onwards in the superior temporal polysensory area (STP). Emotion-specific responses were found after 120 ms in the right anterior areas with right amygdala activation observed only later (approximately 200 ms). Thus, affective blindsight might be mediated by subcortical afferents to temporal areas as suggested in some studies involving non-emotional stimuli. The early activation of the STP in the patient constitutes evidence for fast activation of higher order visual areas in humans despite bilateral V1 destruction. In addition, the absence of awareness of any visual experience in this patient suggests that neither the extrastriate visual areas, nor the prefrontal cortex activation alone are sufficient for conscious perception, which might require recurrent processing within a network of several cerebral areas including V1.

[1]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  M. Yukie,et al.  Direct projection from the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus to the prestriate cortex in macaque monkeys , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[3]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  R. Dolan,et al.  A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating "unseen" fear. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  D. Goodin,et al.  Visual evoked potentials in the investigation of “blindsight” , 1988, Neurology.

[6]  Tony Ro,et al.  Unconscious processing of orientation and color without primary visual cortex. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  Christopher J Rennie,et al.  Mode of Functional Connectivity in Amygdala Pathways Dissociates Level of Awareness for Signals of Fear , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  M. Kerszberg,et al.  A Neuronal Model of a Global Workspace in Effortful Cognitive Tasks , 2001 .

[9]  R Grave de Peralta Menendez,et al.  Imaging the electrical activity of the brain: ELECTRA , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[10]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Fast Backprojections from the Motion to the Primary Visual Area Necessary for Visual Awareness , 2001, Science.

[11]  Luiz Pessoa,et al.  Target visibility and visual awareness modulate amygdala responses to fearful faces. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Frank Tong,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Primary visual cortex and visual awareness , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[13]  D. Amaral,et al.  Some observations on cortical inputs to the macaque monkey amygdala: An anterograde tracing study , 2002, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[14]  A. McDonald Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala , 1998, Progress in Neurobiology.

[15]  Lawrence C. Sincich,et al.  Bypassing V1: a direct geniculate input to area MT , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  T Landis,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for fast visual processing through the human koniocellular pathway when stimuli move. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  A Cohen,et al.  Extrageniculate vision in hemianopic humans: saccade inhibition by signals in the blind field. , 1990, Science.

[18]  Gregor Thut,et al.  Prediction of response speed by anticipatory high‐frequency (gamma band) oscillations in the human brain , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[19]  Harald T Schupp,et al.  Affective blindsight: intact fear conditioning to a visual cue in a cortically blind patient. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  C. Gross,et al.  Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque. II. Effects of superior colliculus removal , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  E. G. Jones,et al.  A projection from the medial pulvinar to the amygdala in primates , 1976, Brain Research.

[22]  D. Thomson,et al.  Spectrum estimation and harmonic analysis , 1982, Proceedings of the IEEE.

[23]  H. Barbas,et al.  Pathways for emotion: interactions of prefrontal and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey , 2002, Neuroscience.

[24]  J B Poline,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  S Zeki,et al.  Conscious visual perception without V1. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Neuronal Coherence as a Mechanism of Effective Corticospinal Interaction , 2005, Science.

[27]  P J Benson,et al.  Cortical evoked potentials due to motion contrast in the blind hemifield. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[28]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. , 1974, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  Robert Tibshirani,et al.  The Elements of Statistical Learning: Data Mining, Inference, and Prediction, 2nd Edition , 2001, Springer Series in Statistics.

[30]  B. Baars IN THE THEATRE OF CONSCIOUSNESS Global Workspace Theory, A Rigorous Scientific Theory of Consciousness. , 1997 .

[31]  R. Adolphs Neural systems for recognizing emotion , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[32]  Victor A. F. Lamme Blindsight: the role of feedforward and feedback corticocortical connections. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[33]  François Mauguière,et al.  Early Amygdala Reaction to Fear Spreading in Occipital, Temporal, and Frontal Cortex A Depth Electrode ERP Study in Human , 2004, Neuron.

[34]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Sustained extrastriate cortical activation without visual awareness revealed by fMRI studies of hemianopic patients , 2001, Vision Research.

[35]  G. Rees,et al.  Neuroimaging: Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  H. Kennedy,et al.  Projection of the lateral geniculate nucleus onto cortical area V2 in the macaque monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  Beatrice de Gelder,et al.  Out of Mind : Varieties of Unconscious Processes , 2001 .

[38]  H. Rodman,et al.  Calbindin immunoreactivity in the geniculo‐extrastriate system of the macaque: Implications for heterogeneity in the koniocellular pathway and recovery from cortical damage , 2001, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[39]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[40]  Asaid Khateb,et al.  Discriminating emotional faces without primary visual cortices involves the right amygdala , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  E. Warrington,et al.  "Blindsight": Vision in a field defect. , 1974, Lancet.

[42]  T. Allison,et al.  Differential Sensitivity of Human Visual Cortex to Faces, Letterstrings, and Textures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  José del R. Millán,et al.  Very high frequency oscillations (VHFO) as a predictor of movement intentions , 2006, NeuroImage.

[44]  L. Weiskrantz Sources of blindsight. , 1993, Science.

[45]  K M Gothard,et al.  Neural responses to facial expression and face identity in the monkey amygdala. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  Tomokatsu Hori,et al.  Comparison of source localization of interictal epileptic spike potentials in patients estimated by the dipole tracing method with the focus directly recorded by the depth electrodes , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[47]  H Spekreijse,et al.  A Neural Correlate of Working Memory in the Monkey Primary Visual Cortex , 2001, Science.

[48]  S. Zeki,et al.  Toward a Theory of Visual Consciousness , 1999, Consciousness and Cognition.

[49]  G. Holmes DISTURBANCES OF VISION BY CEREBRAL LESIONS , 1918, The British journal of ophthalmology.

[50]  T. Poggio,et al.  Object Selectivity of Local Field Potentials and Spikes in the Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2006, Neuron.

[51]  L. Weiskrantz Blindsight : a case study and implications , 1986 .

[52]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Electrical neuroimaging based on biophysical constraints , 2004, NeuroImage.

[53]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Single-neuron responses to emotional visual stimuli recorded in human ventral prefrontal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  R. Grave de Peralta Menendez,et al.  Non‐stationary distributed source approximation: An alternative to improve localization procedures , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[55]  S. Gonzalez-Andino,et al.  A critical analysis of linear inverse solutions to the neuroelectromagnetic inverse problem , 1998, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[56]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Pattern of neuronal activity associated with conscious and unconscious processing of visual signals. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[57]  R. Held,et al.  Residual Visual Function after Brain Wounds involving the Central Visual Pathways in Man , 1973, Nature.

[58]  A. Cowey,et al.  Disruption of visual evoked potentials following a v1 lesion: Implications for blindsight , 2001 .

[59]  G. Rees,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. , 1998, Science.

[60]  L L Chao,et al.  Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces? , 1999, Neuroreport.

[61]  Gregor Thut,et al.  A glimpse into your vision , 2007, Human brain mapping.