Sound localization in a new-world frugivorous bat, Artibeus jamaicensis: acuity, use of binaural cues, and relationship to vision.

Passive sound-localization acuity and its relationship to vision were determined for the echolocating Jamaican fruit bat (Artibeus jamaicensis). A conditioned avoidance procedure was used in which the animals drank fruit juice from a spout in the presence of sounds from their right, but suppressed their behavior, breaking contact with the spout, whenever a sound came from their left, thereby avoiding a mild shock. The mean minimum audible angle for three bats for a 100-ms noise burst was 10 degrees-marginally superior to the 11.6 degrees threshold for Egyptian fruit bats and the 14 degrees threshold for big brown bats. Jamaican fruit bats were also able to localize both low- and high-frequency pure tones, indicating that they can use both binaural phase- and intensity-difference cues to locus. Indeed, their ability to use the binaural phase cue extends up to 6.3 kHz, the highest frequency so far for a mammal. The width of their field of best vision, defined anatomically as the width of the retinal area containing ganglion-cell densities at least 75% of maximum, is 34 degrees. This value is consistent with the previously established relationship between vision and hearing indicating that, even in echolocating bats, the primary function of passive sound localization is to direct the eyes to sound sources.

[1]  J. E. Rose,et al.  Phase-locked response to low-frequency tones in single auditory nerve fibers of the squirrel monkey. , 1967, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Henry E. Heffner,et al.  Evolution of Sound Localization in Mammals , 1992 .

[3]  D. Griffin,et al.  Acoustic orientation of neotropical bats , 1955 .

[4]  Henry E. Heffner,et al.  Behavioral hearing range of the chinchilla , 1991, Hearing Research.

[5]  V. Casagrande,et al.  The size and topographic arrangement of retinal ganglion cells in the galago , 1980, Vision Research.

[6]  H E Heffner,et al.  Sound localization, use of binaural cues and the superior olivary complex in pigs. , 1989, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[7]  R. Heffner Sound localization and the superior olivary complex in horses and cattle , 1981 .

[8]  H. Heffner,et al.  Localization of noise, use of binaural cues, and a description of the superior olivary complex in the smallest carnivore, the least weasel (Mustela nivalis). , 1987, Behavioral neuroscience.

[9]  Gerald H. Jacobs,et al.  Spatial contrast sensitivity in albino and pigmented rats , 1979, Vision Research.

[10]  Henry E. Heffner,et al.  Audiogram of the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus) , 1997, Hearing Research.

[11]  P. Moore Underwater localization of click and pulsed pure-tone signals by the California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). , 1975, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  R. Suthers Optomotor Responses by Echolocating Bats , 1966, Science.

[13]  H. Heffner,et al.  Visual factors in sound localization in mammals , 1992, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[14]  J. Kelly,et al.  Midline and lateral field sound localization in the albino rat (Rattus norvegicus). , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  R. Lindsay,et al.  Listening in the Dark , 1958 .

[16]  G. F. Kuhn Model for the interaural time differences in the azimuthal plane , 1977 .

[17]  H. Heffner,et al.  Hearing in Glires: Domestic rabbit, cotton rat, feral house mouse, and kangaroo rat , 1980 .

[18]  M. Fenton,et al.  What ears do for bats: a comparative study of pinna sound pressure transformation in chiroptera. , 1993, The Journal of experimental biology.

[19]  J. Driver,et al.  Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  A. Novick,et al.  THE SENSITIVITY OF ECHOLOCATION IN THE FRUIT BAT, ROUSETTUS , 1958 .

[21]  H. Heffner,et al.  Sound localization in an Old-World fruit bat (Rousettus aegyptiacus): acuity, use of binaural cues, and relationship to vision. , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.

[22]  John R. Cotter Retinofugal projections of the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus and the neotropical fruit bat, Artibeus jamaicensis. , 1985, The American journal of anatomy.

[23]  Henry E. Heffner,et al.  Sound localization in an Old-World fruit bat (Rousettus aegyptiacus): acuity, use of binaural cues, and relationship to vision. , 1999 .

[24]  H. Heffner,et al.  Hearing in a megachiropteran fruit bat (Rousettus aegyptiacus). , 1998, Journal of comparative psychology.

[25]  H. Heffner,et al.  Passive sound-localization ability of the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus) , 1998, Hearing Research.

[26]  H. Heffner,et al.  Vestigial hearing in a fossorial mammal, the pocket gopher (Geomys bursarius) , 1990, Hearing Research.

[27]  F. Bonaccorso,et al.  FEEDING BEHAVIOUR AND FORAGING STRATEGIES OF CAPTIVE PHYLLOSTOMID FRUIT BATS: AN EXPERIMENTAL STUDY , 1987 .

[28]  H. Heffner,et al.  Degenerate hearing and sound localization in naked mole rats (Heterocephalus glaber), with an overview of central auditory structures , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[29]  K Saberi,et al.  How do owls localize interaurally phase-ambiguous signals? , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  Hans-Ulrich Schnitzler,et al.  Performance of Airborne Animal Sonar Systems: I. Microchiroptera , 1980 .

[31]  J. Pettigrew,et al.  Peak density and distribution of ganglion cells in the retinae of microchiropteran bats: implications for visual acuity. , 1988, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[32]  M. Fenton,et al.  Flight and echlocation in the ecology and evolution of bats. , 1997, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[33]  Henry E. Heffner,et al.  Sound localization in wild Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus) , 1985, Hearing Research.

[34]  E. Kalko,et al.  Relation of fig fruit characteristics to fruit‐eating bats in the New and Old World tropics , 1996 .

[35]  G. Langner,et al.  Neural processing and representation of periodicity pitch. , 1997, Acta oto-laryngologica. Supplementum.

[36]  H. Heffner,et al.  Sound localization acuity in the cat: Effect of azimuth, signal duration, and test procedure , 1988, Hearing Research.

[37]  G. C. Thompson,et al.  Neuroanatomical basis of binaural phase-difference analysis for sound localization: a comparative study. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[38]  Henry E. Heffner,et al.  Hearing and sound localization in blind mole rats (Spalax ehrenbergi) , 1992, Hearing Research.

[39]  D. Houben,et al.  Auditory lateralization in monkeys: an examination of two cues serving directional hearing. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[40]  Henry E. Heffner,et al.  Hearing in a Megachiropteran Fruit Bat (Rousettus aegyptiacus) , 1998 .

[41]  H. Heffner,et al.  Sound localization in chinchillas. I: Left/right discriminations , 1994, Hearing Research.

[42]  R. G. Klumpp,et al.  Some Measurements of Interaural Time Difference Thresholds , 1956 .

[43]  H. Heffner,et al.  A syringe-pump food-paste dispenser , 1990 .

[44]  J. Kelly,et al.  Effects of auditory cortical lesions on sound localization by the rat. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  A. Palmer,et al.  Phase-locking in the cochlear nerve of the guinea-pig and its relation to the receptor potential of inner hair-cells , 1986, Hearing Research.

[46]  Jerry V. Tobias,et al.  Effective Onset Duration of Auditory Stimuli , 1959 .

[47]  M. Brock Fenton,et al.  Visual acuity, sensitivity and binocularity in a gleaning insectivorous bat, Macrotus californicus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae) , 1986, Animal Behaviour.