Why do beliefs about intelligence influence learning success? A social cognitive neuroscience model.

Students' beliefs and goals can powerfully influence their learning success. Those who believe intelligence is a fixed entity (entity theorists) tend to emphasize 'performance goals,' leaving them vulnerable to negative feedback and likely to disengage from challenging learning opportunities. In contrast, students who believe intelligence is malleable (incremental theorists) tend to emphasize 'learning goals' and rebound better from occasional failures. Guided by cognitive neuroscience models of top-down, goal-directed behavior, we use event-related potentials (ERPs) to understand how these beliefs influence attention to information associated with successful error correction. Focusing on waveforms associated with conflict detection and error correction in a test of general knowledge, we found evidence indicating that entity theorists oriented differently toward negative performance feedback, as indicated by an enhanced anterior frontal P3 that was also positively correlated with concerns about proving ability relative to others. Yet, following negative feedback, entity theorists demonstrated less sustained memory-related activity (left temporal negativity) to corrective information, suggesting reduced effortful conceptual encoding of this material-a strategic approach that may have contributed to their reduced error correction on a subsequent surprise retest. These results suggest that beliefs can influence learning success through top-down biasing of attention and conceptual processing toward goal-congruent information.

[1]  C. Dweck,et al.  A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality , 1988 .

[2]  Robert F. Simons,et al.  Anxiety and error-related brain activity , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[3]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Dissociable prefrontal brain systems for attention and emotion , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Successful declarative memory formation is associated with ongoing activity during encoding in a distributed neocortical network related to working memory: A magnetoencephalography study , 2006, Neuroscience.

[6]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[7]  K. Paller,et al.  Observing the transformation of experience into memory , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[9]  W. Gehring,et al.  Medial Frontal Cortex Activity and Loss-Related Responses to Errors , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  R. West,et al.  Adjustments of Cognitive Control in Younger and Older Adults , 2005, Cortex.

[12]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[13]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[14]  C. Dweck,et al.  Implicit theories, attributions, and coping : A meaning system approach , 1999 .

[15]  Karen J. Mathewson,et al.  Brain bases of error-related ERPs as influenced by age and task , 2005, Biological Psychology.

[16]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Working Memory Maintenance Contributes to Long-term Memory Formation: Neural and Behavioral Evidence , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  Andrew D. Lawrence,et al.  Individual Differences in the Modulation of Fear-Related Brain Activation by Attentional Control , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Attentional systems in target and distractor processing: a combined ERP and fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[19]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[20]  Tobias Egner,et al.  The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task , 2005, NeuroImage.

[21]  Lisa S. Blackwell,et al.  Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: a longitudinal study and an intervention. , 2007, Child development.

[22]  R T Knight,et al.  Anatomic bases of event-related potentials and their relationship to novelty detection in humans. , 1998, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[23]  Ray Johnson,et al.  On why the elderly have normal semantic retrieval but deficient episodic encoding: A study of left inferior frontal ERP activity , 2006, NeuroImage.

[24]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  Neural correlates of error detection and correction in a semantic retrieval task. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[25]  Jim Blascovich,et al.  Using physiological indexes of psychological processes in social psychological research. , 2000 .

[26]  M. Mesulam,et al.  The central role of the prefrontal cortex in directing attention to novel events. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  E. Harmon-Jones,et al.  Neural Signals for the Detection of Unintentional Race Bias , 2004, Psychological science.

[28]  D Friedman,et al.  Event‐related potential (ERP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: A selective review , 2000, Microscopy research and technique.

[29]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  R. West,et al.  Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  A. Nobre,et al.  The anatomy and time course of semantic priming investigated by fMRI and ERPs , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Daniel C Molden,et al.  Finding "meaning" in psychology: a lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. , 2006, The American psychologist.

[33]  C. Dweck,et al.  Implicit Theories and Evaluative Processes in Person Cognition , 1997 .

[34]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[35]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[36]  Carol S. Dweck,et al.  Motivational Effects on Attention, Cognition, and Performance , 2004 .

[37]  B. Vogt,et al.  Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Reinforcement-related brain potentials from medial frontal cortex: origins and functional significance , 2004, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[39]  C. Dweck,et al.  Clarifying achievement goals and their impact. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  M I Posner,et al.  Neuroanatomy, circuitry and plasticity of word reading. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[41]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  Errors committed with high confidence are hypercorrected. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[42]  Jeffrey C. Cooper,et al.  Individual differences in trait rumination and the neural systems supporting cognitive reappraisal , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[43]  T. Allison,et al.  Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe , 1994, Nature.

[44]  M. Mather,et al.  Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  T. Braver,et al.  Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. , 2007 .

[46]  J. Allen,et al.  Error monitoring during reward and avoidance learning in high- and low-socialized individuals. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[47]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  ERP correlates of error monitoring in 10-year olds are related to socialization , 2005, Biological Psychology.

[48]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Coherent theta-band EEG activity predicts item-context binding during encoding , 2005, NeuroImage.

[49]  C. Dweck,et al.  Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[51]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Where and When the Anterior Cingulate Cortex Modulates Attentional Response: Combined fMRI and ERP Evidence , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  F. Craik,et al.  The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[53]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: II. Effects of word type and semantic priming , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[54]  C. Dweck,et al.  Praise for intelligence can undermine children's motivation and performance. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[55]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[56]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  R. Simons,et al.  Error-related psychophysiology and negative affect , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[58]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[59]  D. Tucker,et al.  Mood, personality, and self-monitoring: negative affect and emotionality in relation to frontal lobe mechanisms of error monitoring. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[60]  E. Halgren,et al.  Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. III. Frontal cortex. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[61]  D. Friedman,et al.  The novelty P3: an event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain's evaluation of novelty , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[62]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  An Electrophysiological Investigation of Preparatory Attentional Control in a Spatial Stroop Task , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  Stefan R Schweinberger,et al.  N200, N250r, and N400 event-related brain potentials reveal three loci of repetition priming for familiar names. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[64]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[65]  T W Picton,et al.  Attention and successful episodic encoding: an event-related potential study. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.