Children’s affective involvement in early word learning

[1]  Robert Hepach,et al.  Young children show negative emotions after failing to help others , 2022, PloS one.

[2]  Bahar Köymen,et al.  Preschool children's evaluations of their own unjustified requests. , 2022, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[3]  Robert Hepach,et al.  How being observed influences preschoolers’ emotions following (less) deserving help , 2022, Social Development.

[4]  Robert Hepach,et al.  Cultural values shape the expression of self-evaluative social emotions , 2021, Scientific Reports.

[5]  Chang Huan Lo,et al.  Word learning from a tablet app: Toddlers perform better in a passive context , 2020, PloS one.

[6]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children show positive emotions when seeing someone get the help they deserve , 2020 .

[7]  D. Haun,et al.  The ontogeny of children's social emotions in response to (un)fairness , 2020, Royal Society Open Science.

[8]  N. Mani,et al.  Children learn words easier when they are interested in the category to which the word belongs. , 2020, Developmental science.

[9]  I. Dobbins,et al.  Pupil dilation during memory encoding reflects time pressure rather than depth of processing. , 2020, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  T. Griffiths,et al.  Reconciling novelty and complexity through a rational analysis of curiosity. , 2019, Psychological review.

[11]  Tanya Behne,et al.  Retrospective inferences in selective trust , 2019, Royal Society Open Science.

[12]  Katherine E. Twomey,et al.  The Limits of Infants’ Early Word Learning , 2019, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.

[13]  N. Mani,et al.  Why Do Children Learn the Words They Do , 2018 .

[14]  A. Rodríguez-Fornells,et al.  Intrinsically regulated learning is modulated by synaptic dopamine signaling , 2018, eLife.

[15]  Makeba Parramore Wilbourn,et al.  Communicating to Learn: Infants' Pointing Gestures Result in Optimal Learning. , 2018, Child development.

[16]  A. Castel,et al.  The Role of Interest in Memory for Trivia Questions: An Investigation With a Large-Scale Database , 2017, Motivation science.

[17]  M. Friedrich ERP indices of word learning : What do they reflect and what do they tell us about the neural representations of early words? , 2017 .

[18]  Katherine E. Twomey,et al.  Curiosity‐based learning in infants: a neurocomputational approach , 2017, Developmental science.

[19]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young Children Want to See Others Get the Help They Need. , 2016, Child development.

[20]  Jari Lavonen,et al.  Integrating the light and dark sides of student engagement using person-oriented and situation-specific approaches , 2016 .

[21]  Pierre-Yves Oudeyer,et al.  How Evolution May Work Through Curiosity-Driven Developmental Process , 2016, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[22]  G. Westermann,et al.  Pupillometry in Infancy Research , 2016 .

[23]  W. Fitch,et al.  The Eye is Listening: Music-Induced Arousal and Individual Differences Predict Pupillary Responses , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[24]  B. Hayden,et al.  The Psychology and Neuroscience of Curiosity , 2015, Neuron.

[25]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Novel paradigms to measure variability of behavior in early childhood: posture, gaze, and pupil dilation , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[26]  D. Berlyne Conflict, arousal, and curiosity , 2014 .

[27]  H. Heinze,et al.  The Role of Reward in Word Learning and Its Implications for Language Acquisition , 2014, Current Biology.

[28]  Matthias J. Gruber,et al.  States of Curiosity Modulate Hippocampus-Dependent Learning via the Dopaminergic Circuit , 2014, Neuron.

[29]  T. Gliga,et al.  Infants Learn What They Want to Learn: Responding to Infant Pointing Leads to Superior Learning , 2014, PloS one.

[30]  K. Fiedler,et al.  International handbook of emotions in education. , 2014 .

[31]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children sympathize less in response to unjustified emotional distress. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[32]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young Children Are Intrinsically Motivated to See Others Helped , 2012, Psychological science.

[33]  Richard N. Aslin,et al.  The Goldilocks Effect: Human Infants Allocate Attention to Visual Sequences That Are Neither Too Simple Nor Too Complex , 2012, PloS one.

[34]  D. Swingley,et al.  At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Thirteen- and 18-month-old Infants Recognize When They Need Referential Information , 2011 .

[36]  Barbara L Finlay,et al.  Thinking outside the cortex: social motivation in the evolution and development of language. , 2010, Developmental science.

[37]  E. Jansen,et al.  ‘It’s almost a mindset that teachers need to change’: first‐year students’ need to be inducted into time management , 2010 .

[38]  M. Guitart-Masip,et al.  NOvelty-related Motivation of Anticipation and exploration by Dopamine (NOMAD): Implications for healthy aging , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[39]  H. Aarts,et al.  The Unconscious Eye Opener , 2009, Psychological science.

[40]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  The Wick in the Candle of Learning , 2009, Psychological science.

[41]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  The Wick in the Candle of Learning: Epistemic Curiosity Activates Reward Circuitry and Enhances Memory , 2008 .

[42]  T. Kindermann,et al.  Engagement and Disaffection in the Classroom: Part of a Larger Motivational Dynamic? , 2008 .

[43]  M. Bradley,et al.  The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[44]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Dissociating the Role of the Orbitofrontal Cortex and the Striatum in the Computation of Goal Values and Prediction Errors , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics , 2008, Cognition.

[46]  Bruno Laeng,et al.  Women's pupillary responses to sexually significant others during the hormonal cycle , 2007, Hormones and Behavior.

[47]  R. Pekrun The Control-Value Theory of Achievement Emotions: Assumptions, Corollaries, and Implications for Educational Research and Practice , 2006 .

[48]  J. Changeux,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.5 May 2006 Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy , 2022 .

[49]  P. Harris,et al.  Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. , 2005, Child development.

[50]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Pupillary Responses With and Without Awareness in Blindsight , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.

[51]  G. Loewenstein The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation. , 1994 .

[52]  M. Lewis,et al.  Differences in shame and pride as a function of children's gender and task difficulty. , 1992, Child development.

[53]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  The Ferrier Lecture, 1989 - Outlooks for blindsight: explicit methodologies for implicit processes , 1990, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B. Biological Sciences.

[54]  D Kahneman,et al.  Pupil Diameter and Load on Memory , 1966, Science.

[55]  E. Hess,et al.  Pupil Size in Relation to Mental Activity during Simple Problem-Solving , 1964, Science.

[56]  D. Berlyne A theory of human curiosity. , 1954, British journal of psychology.

[57]  M. Sabbagh,et al.  Mechanisms of Selective Word Learning: Evidence and Implications , 2018 .

[58]  M. Tomasello,et al.  The Fulfillment of Others’ Needs Elevates Children’s Body Posture , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[59]  Celeste Kidd,et al.  Young Children’s Self-Directed Information Gathering on Touchscreens , 2015 .

[60]  S. Sirois,et al.  Pupillometry , 2012, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[61]  E. Deci,et al.  Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. , 2000, The American psychologist.