Category-based inhibition of focused attention across consecutive trials.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E Donchin,et al. A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.
[2] Brian A. Goolsby,et al. Adaptive modulation of color salience contingent upon global form coding and task relevance , 2005, Vision Research.
[3] H. Bülthoff,et al. The contribution of different facial regions to the recognition of conversational expressions. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[4] S. Hillyard,et al. Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[5] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection , 1999, Nature.
[6] H. Heinze,et al. An event-related brain potential study of visual selective attention to conjunctions of color and shape. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[7] K Nakayama,et al. Visual attention to surfaces in three-dimensional space. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[8] J. Snel,et al. The effects of caffeine on visual selective attention to color: an ERP study. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[9] S. Hillyard,et al. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[10] B. Bartholow,et al. Effects of alcohol sensitivity on P3 event-related potential reactivity to alcohol cues. , 2007, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.
[11] S. Tipper. The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[12] Piotr Jaskowski,et al. Evidence for an Integrative Role of P3b in Linking Reaction to Perception , 2005 .
[13] Robert Oostenveld,et al. The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[14] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of popout: III. A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection , 2000 .
[15] W. Mak,et al. Attentional Bias Associated with Habitual Self-Stigma in People with Mental Illness , 2015, PloS one.
[16] Alejandro Lleras,et al. Inter-trial inhibition of attention to features is modulated by task relevance. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[17] M. Wertheimer,et al. A source book of Gestalt psychology. , 1939 .
[18] Edi Suharto,et al. Spatio-Temporal Analysis , 2015 .
[19] S. Luck,et al. Bridging the Gap between Monkey Neurophysiology and Human Perception: An Ambiguity Resolution Theory of Visual Selective Attention , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.
[20] G. McCarthy,et al. Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.
[21] S. Hillyard,et al. Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] J. Kawahara,et al. Task relevance and response suppression in the distractor previewing effect , 2004 .
[23] S. Hillyard,et al. Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.
[24] S. Luck,et al. How does attention attenuate target-distractor interference in vision?. Evidence from magnetoencephalographic recordings. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[25] M. Wertheimer. Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt. II , 1923 .
[26] J. Polich. Updating P 300 : An Integrative Theory of P 3 a and P 3 b , 2009 .
[27] Alejandro Lleras,et al. Saccadic repulsion in pop-out search: how a target's dodgy history can push the eyes away from it. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[28] J. Polich. Neuropsychology of P300 , 2011 .
[29] Alejandro Lleras,et al. The remains of the trial: goal-determined inter-trial suppression of selective attention. , 2009, Progress in brain research.
[30] H. Semlitsch,et al. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[31] C D Wickens,et al. An Analysis of the Processing Requirements of a Complex Perceptual-Motor Task , 1983, Human factors.
[32] Guillaume A Rousselet,et al. Inverting faces elicits sensitivity to race on the N170 component: a cross-cultural study. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[33] S. Luck,et al. Neural sources of focused attention in visual search. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.
[34] H. Bülthoff,et al. The effect of cognition on the visually-induced illusion of self-motion (vection) , 2004 .
[35] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Handbook of Psychophysiology: Event-Related Brain Potentials: Methods, Theory, and Applications , 2007 .
[36] C. Koch. Strategies and Models of Selective Attention , 2010 .
[37] Geoffrey F Woodman,et al. Serial deployment of attention during visual search. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[38] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[40] R. Rafal,et al. Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[41] E. Donchin,et al. P300 and tracking difficulty: evidence for multiple resources in dual-task performance. , 1980, Psychophysiology.
[42] Jeff Miller,et al. Measurement of ERP latency differences: a comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods. , 2008, Psychophysiology.
[43] Alejandro Lleras,et al. Intertrial inhibition of focused attention in pop-out search , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.
[44] J. Theeuwes. Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[45] I. Syed,et al. The effects of caffeine. , 1976, Journal of the American Pharmaceutical Association.
[46] L. Sobell,et al. Reliability of a family tree questionnaire for assessing family history of alcohol problems. , 1985, Drug and alcohol dependence.
[47] T. Duka,et al. Attentional bias associated with alcohol cues: differences between heavy and occasional social drinkers , 2001, Psychopharmacology.
[48] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of popout: II. Role of position , 1996 .
[49] S J Luck,et al. Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[50] B. Bartholow,et al. Specificity of P3 event-related potential reactivity to alcohol cues in individuals low in alcohol sensitivity. , 2010, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.
[51] E. Donchin. Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.
[52] A. Lleras,et al. The effect of feature discriminability on the intertrial inhibition of focused attention , 2010 .
[53] S. Hillyard,et al. Spatio-temporal analysis of feature-based attention. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.
[54] Jun-ichiro Kawahara,et al. The perceptual and cognitive distractor-previewing effect. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[55] A Kok,et al. Event-related potentials to conjunctions of spatial frequency and orientation as a function of stimulus parameters and response requirements. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[56] M Eimer,et al. Spatial cueing, sensory gating and selective response preparation: an ERP study on visuo-spatial orienting. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[57] S. Treue,et al. Feature-Based Attention Increases the Selectivity of Population Responses in Primate Visual Cortex , 2004, Current Biology.
[58] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[59] J. Hopfinger,et al. Electrophysiological evidence of alcohol-related attentional bias in social drinkers low in alcohol sensitivity. , 2010, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.
[60] F E Bloom,et al. Meta-analysis of P300 amplitude from males at risk for alcoholism. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[61] R. Heuser. Surprise, surprise , 2014, Catheterization and cardiovascular interventions : official journal of the Society for Cardiac Angiography & Interventions.
[62] Elkan G. Akyürek,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of detecting a visual target and detecting its absence: The role of feature dimensions , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[63] Alejandro Lleras,et al. Electrophysiological evidence of feature-based inhibition of focused attention across consecutive trials. , 2008, Psychophysiology.
[64] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[65] M. Eimer. “Sensory gating” as a mechanism for visuospatial orienting: Electrophysiological evidence from trial-by-trial cuing experiments , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[66] Jeff Miller,et al. Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[67] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[68] J. Duncan. Selective attention and the organization of visual information. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.