Attentional But Not Pre-Attentive Neural Measures of Auditory Discrimination Are Atypical in Children With Developmental Language Disorder
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E. Grigorenko,et al. Spelling well despite developmental language disorder: what makes it possible? , 2013, Annals of Dyslexia.
[2] E. Walker,et al. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .
[3] E. Grigorenko,et al. Gender and agreement processing in children with Developmental Language Disorder* , 2013, Journal of Child Language.
[4] Joseph Dien,et al. Applying Principal Components Analysis to Event-Related Potentials: A Tutorial , 2012, Developmental neuropsychology.
[5] D. Bishop,et al. Auditory Deficit as a Consequence Rather than Endophenotype of Specific Language Impairment: Electrophysiological Evidence , 2012, PloS one.
[6] Joseph T. Chang,et al. The language phenotype of a small geographically isolated Russian-speaking population: Implications for genetic and clinical studies of developmental language disorder , 2012, Applied Psycholinguistics.
[7] Dorothy V.M. Bishop,et al. Co-localisation of abnormal brain structure and function in specific language impairment , 2012, Brain and Language.
[8] Theodore A. Walls,et al. Investigating the effects of syllable complexity in Russian-speaking children with SLI. , 2011, Journal of child language.
[9] P. Jannin,et al. Abnormal functional lateralization and activity of language brain areas in typical specific language impairment (developmental dysphasia). , 2011, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[10] Elham Ahmed Shaheen,et al. Relation between language, audio-vocal psycholinguistic abilities and P300 in children having specific language impairment. , 2011, International journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology.
[11] E. Grigorenko,et al. The relationship between syntactic development and Theory of Mind: Evidence from a small-population study of a developmental language disorder , 2011, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[12] Seth D. Pollak,et al. P300 as a measure of processing capacity in auditory and visual domains in specific language impairment , 2011, Brain Research.
[13] P. Hagoort,et al. The nature of auditory discrimination problems in children with specific language impairment: An MMN study , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[14] J. Tomblin,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of rapid auditory and linguistic processing in adolescents with specific language impairment , 2010, Brain and Language.
[15] E. Grigorenko,et al. Etiologies and Molecular Mechanisms of Communication Disorders , 2010, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.
[16] Joseph Dien,et al. The ERP PCA Toolkit: An open source program for advanced statistical analysis of event-related potential data , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[17] R. Plomin,et al. Common disorders are quantitative traits , 2009, Nature Reviews Genetics.
[18] M. Seghier,et al. The left superior temporal gyrus is a shared substrate for auditory short-term memory and speech comprehension: evidence from 210 patients with stroke , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[19] Carmen M. Atkinson,et al. Atypical brain responses to sounds in children with specific language and reading impairments. , 2009, Developmental science.
[20] N. Boutros,et al. P50, N100, and P200 sensory gating: relationships with behavioral inhibition, attention, and working memory. , 2009, Psychophysiology.
[21] D. Bishop,et al. Mismatch Response to Polysyllabic Nonwords: A Neurophysiological Signature of Language Learning Capacity , 2009, PloS one.
[22] Julia L. Evans,et al. Complex sentence comprehension and working memory in children with specific language impairment. , 2009, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[23] Kirsten Abbot-Smith,et al. German children's comprehension of word order and case marking in causative sentences. , 2008, Child development.
[24] M. Matousek,et al. Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA) in the prediction of response to cholinesterase inhibitors in patients with Alzheimer's disease , 2008 .
[25] R. Näätänen,et al. The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[26] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[27] D. Bishop,et al. Atypical long-latency auditory event-related potentials in a subset of children with specific language impairment , 2007, Developmental science.
[28] J. Polich,et al. Normative Variation of P3a and P3b from a Large Sample , 2007 .
[29] D. Bishop,et al. Using mismatch negativity to study central auditory processing in developmental language and literacy impairments: where are we, and where should we be going? , 2007, Psychological bulletin.
[30] Katharine Graf Estes,et al. Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: a meta-analysis. , 2007, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[31] Z. Gou,et al. The infant as a prelinguistic model for language learning impairments: Predicting from event-related potentials to behavior , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[32] S. Gray,et al. The relationship between phonological memory, receptive vocabulary, and fast mapping in young children with specific language impairment. , 2006, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[33] F. Pulvermüller,et al. Language outside the focus of attention: The mismatch negativity as a tool for studying higher cognitive processes , 2006, Progress in Neurobiology.
[34] Tyrone D. Cannon,et al. Endophenotypes in the genetic analyses of mental disorders. , 2006, Annual review of clinical psychology.
[35] Dorothy V. M. Bishop,et al. Individual Differences in Auditory Processing in Specific Language Impairment: A Follow-Up Study using Event-Related Potentials and Behavioural Thresholds , 2005, Cortex.
[36] K. Eklund,et al. Brain Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) Measured at Birth Predict Later Language Development in Children with and Without Familial Risk for Dyslexia , 2005, Cortex.
[37] D. Linden. The P300: Where in the Brain Is It Produced and What Does It Tell Us? , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[38] Salil H. Patel,et al. Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected Studies of the Event-Related Potential , 2005, International journal of medical sciences.
[39] G. McArthur,et al. Speech and non-speech processing in people with specific language impairment: A behavioural and electrophysiological study , 2005, Brain and Language.
[40] Angela D. Friederici,et al. Brain Signatures of Syntactic and Semantic Processes during Children's Language Development , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[41] K. Crowley,et al. A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[42] D. V. M. Bishop,et al. Which People with Specific Language Impairment have Auditory Processing Deficits? , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[43] R. Uwer,et al. Automatic processing of tones and speech stimuli in children with specific language impairment. , 2002, Developmental medicine and child neurology.
[44] E. Golob,et al. Auditory event-related potentials during target detection are abnormal in mild cognitive impairment , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[45] Heikki Lyytinen,et al. Event-Related Potentials and Consonant Differentiation in Newborns with Familial Risk for Dyslexia , 2001, Journal of learning disabilities.
[46] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[47] L B Leonard,et al. Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[48] K. Juottonen,et al. Abnormal Frequency Mismatch Negativity in Mentally Retarded Children and in Children With Developmental Dysphasia , 1998, Journal of child neurology.
[49] L. Elias,et al. Footedness is a better predictor than is handedness of emotional lateralization , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[50] J. Tomblin,et al. Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. , 1997, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[51] T. Carrell,et al. Auditory Neurophysiologic Responses and Discrimination Deficits in Children with Learning Problems , 1996, Science.
[52] S. Gathercole,et al. Phonological Working Memory and Spoken Language Development in Young Children , 1996 .
[53] O. Aaltonen,et al. Is there a late-latency mismatch negativity (MMN) component? , 1995 .
[54] P. Korpilahti,et al. Auditory ERP components and mismatch negativity in dysphasic children. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[55] E. Courchesne,et al. Pathophysiologic findings in nonretarded autism and receptive developmental language disorder , 1989, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.
[56] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[57] R. Verleger. Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3 , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[58] P. Tallal,et al. Speech acoustic-cue discrimination abilities of normally developing and language-impaired children. , 1981, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[59] Jessica Daecher,et al. Frog Where Are You , 2016 .
[60] M. Carter. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. , 2014 .
[61] E. Grigorenko,et al. Morphology and developmental language disorders: new tools for Russian , 2012 .
[62] Austin F. Frank,et al. Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R , 2010 .
[63] Gena McKinley. Free Fall , 2006, Annals of Internal Medicine.
[64] K. Takemasa,et al. An event-related potential study on the impairment of automatic processing of auditory input in schizophrenia , 2005, Brain Topography.
[65] R D Pascual-Marqui,et al. Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. , 2002, Methods and findings in experimental and clinical pharmacology.
[66] I. Rosén,et al. Auditory event-related brain potentials in children with specific language impairment. , 2002, European journal of paediatric neurology : EJPN : official journal of the European Paediatric Neurology Society.
[67] R. Näätänen. The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[68] Mark E. Pflieger,et al. Theory of a Spatial Filter for Removing Ocular Artifacts With Preservation of EEG , 2001 .
[69] A. Baddeley,et al. The phonological loop as a language learning device. , 1998, Psychological review.
[70] E L Grigorenko,et al. Susceptibility loci for distinct components of developmental dyslexia on chromosomes 6 and 15. , 1997, American journal of human genetics.
[71] R Näätänen,et al. Mismatch negativity--a unique measure of sensory processing in audition. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.
[72] E. Courchesne,et al. Increased amplitude of the auditory P2 and P3b components in adolescents with developmental dysphasia. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.
[73] P Tallal,et al. Developmental aphasia: rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. , 1974, Neuropsychologia.
[74] Mercer Mayer,et al. Frog, Where Are You? , 1969 .