Synapsin Determines Memory Strength after Punishment- and Relief-Learning

Adverse life events can induce two kinds of memory with opposite valence, dependent on timing: “negative” memories for stimuli preceding them and “positive” memories for stimuli experienced at the moment of “relief.” Such punishment memory and relief memory are found in insects, rats, and man. For example, fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) avoid an odor after odor-shock training (“forward conditioning” of the odor), whereas after shock-odor training (“backward conditioning” of the odor) they approach it. Do these timing-dependent associative processes share molecular determinants? We focus on the role of Synapsin, a conserved presynaptic phosphoprotein regulating the balance between the reserve pool and the readily releasable pool of synaptic vesicles. We find that a lack of Synapsin leaves task-relevant sensory and motor faculties unaffected. In contrast, both punishment memory and relief memory scores are reduced. These defects reflect a true lessening of associative memory strength, as distortions in nonassociative processing (e.g., susceptibility to handling, adaptation, habituation, sensitization), discrimination ability, and changes in the time course of coincidence detection can be ruled out as alternative explanations. Reductions in punishment- and relief-memory strength are also observed upon an RNAi-mediated knock-down of Synapsin, and are rescued both by acutely restoring Synapsin and by locally restoring it in the mushroom bodies of mutant flies. Thus, both punishment memory and relief memory require the Synapsin protein and in this sense share genetic and molecular determinants. We note that corresponding molecular commonalities between punishment memory and relief memory in humans would constrain pharmacological attempts to selectively interfere with excessive associative punishment memories, e.g., after traumatic experiences.

[1]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  Identification of Genes That Promote or Inhibit Olfactory Memory Formation in Drosophila , 2015, Genetics.

[2]  G. Rubin,et al.  The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning , 2014, eLife.

[3]  G. Rubin,et al.  Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila , 2014, eLife.

[4]  M. Fendt,et al.  Relief learning is distinguished from safety learning by the requirement of the nucleus accumbens , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[5]  E. Navratilova,et al.  Reward and motivation in pain and pain relief , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  J. Littleton,et al.  Synapsin Regulates Activity-Dependent Outgrowth of Synaptic Boutons at the Drosophila Neuromuscular Junction , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Scott Waddell,et al.  Drosophila Learn Opposing Components of a Compound Food Stimulus , 2014, Current Biology.

[8]  Paul Szyszka,et al.  Converging Circuits Mediate Temperature and Shock Aversive Olfactory Conditioning in Drosophila , 2014, Current Biology.

[9]  S. Tomchik,et al.  Dopaminergic Modulation of cAMP Drives Nonlinear Plasticity across the Drosophila Mushroom Body Lobes , 2014, Current Biology.

[10]  M. Fendt,et al.  Pain-relief learning in flies, rats, and man: basic research and applied perspectives , 2014, Learning & memory.

[11]  A. R. Wagner SOP: A Model of Automatic Memory Processing in Animal Behavior , 2014 .

[12]  A. Fiala,et al.  Differential Associative Training Enhances Olfactory Acuity in Drosophila melanogaster , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Johannes Felsenberg,et al.  Short- and long-term memories formed upon backward conditioning in honeybees (Apis mellifera) , 2013, Learning & memory.

[14]  André Fiala,et al.  Localization of the Contacts Between Kenyon Cells and Aminergic Neurons in the Drosophila melanogaster Brain Using SplitGFP Reconstitution , 2013, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[15]  T. Préat,et al.  Two pairs of mushroom body efferent neurons are required for appetitive long-term memory retrieval in Drosophila. , 2013, Cell reports.

[16]  Mani Ramaswami,et al.  Synapsin Function in GABA-ergic Interneurons Is Required for Short-Term Olfactory Habituation , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Andrew C. Lin,et al.  Different Kenyon Cell Populations Drive Learned Approach and Avoidance in Drosophila , 2013, Neuron.

[18]  B. Gerber,et al.  Memory decay and susceptibility to amnesia dissociate punishment- from relief-learning , 2013, Biology Letters.

[19]  F. Benfenati,et al.  Autism-related behavioral abnormalities in synapsin knockout mice , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  Robert A. A. Campbell,et al.  Imaging a Population Code for Odor Identity in the Drosophila Mushroom Body , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  Bertram Gerber,et al.  Maggot learning and Synapsin function , 2013, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[22]  M. Prinstein,et al.  The Nature of Pain Offset Relief in Nonsuicidal Self-Injury , 2013 .

[23]  M. Fendt,et al.  Onset and offset of aversive events establish distinct memories requiring fear and reward networks. , 2012, Learning & memory.

[24]  R. Menzel The honeybee as a model for understanding the basis of cognition , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  Daryl M. Gohl,et al.  Layered reward signaling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila , 2012, Nature.

[26]  G. Rubin,et al.  A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila , 2012, Nature.

[27]  Yoshinori Aso,et al.  Three Dopamine Pathways Induce Aversive Odor Memories with Different Stability , 2012, PLoS genetics.

[28]  Michael F. Lin,et al.  Evidence of abundant stop codon readthrough in Drosophila and other metazoa. , 2011, Genome research.

[29]  M. Timme,et al.  A Combined Perceptual, Physico-Chemical, and Imaging Approach to ‘Odour-Distances’ Suggests a Categorizing Function of the Drosophila Antennal Lobe , 2011, PloS one.

[30]  G. Rubin,et al.  Mushroom body efferent neurons responsible for aversive olfactory memory retrieval in Drosophila , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  B. Gerber,et al.  Cellular site and molecular mode of synapsin action in associative learning. , 2011, Learning & memory.

[32]  E. Buchner,et al.  Mass spectrometric analysis of synapsins in Drosophila melanogaster and identification of novel phosphorylation sites. , 2011, Journal of proteome research.

[33]  B. Gerber,et al.  A Neurogenetic Dissociation between Punishment-, Reward-, and Relief-Learning in Drosophila , 2010, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[34]  Yoshinori Aso,et al.  Specific Dopaminergic Neurons for the Formation of Labile Aversive Memory , 2010, Current Biology.

[35]  P. Pauli,et al.  A rift between implicit and explicit conditioned valence in human pain relief learning , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  Thomas Preat,et al.  PKA Dynamics in a Drosophila Learning Center: Coincidence Detection by Rutabaga Adenylyl Cyclase and Spatial Regulation by Dunce Phosphodiesterase , 2010, Neuron.

[37]  B. Gerber,et al.  Synapsin is selectively required for anesthesia-sensitive memory. , 2010, Learning & memory.

[38]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  Dynamics of Learning-Related cAMP Signaling and Stimulus Integration in the Drosophila Olfactory Pathway , 2009, Neuron.

[39]  P. Greengard,et al.  Writing Memories with Light-Addressable Reinforcement Circuitry , 2009, Cell.

[40]  B. Gerber,et al.  Odour intensity learning in fruit flies , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[41]  Wanhe Li,et al.  Short- and Long-Term Memory in Drosophila Require cAMP Signaling in Distinct Neuron Types , 2009, Current Biology.

[42]  Andreas S. Thum,et al.  The Role of Dopamine in Drosophila Larval Classical Olfactory Conditioning , 2009, PloS one.

[43]  Yoshinori Aso,et al.  The Mushroom Body of Adult Drosophila Characterized by GAL4 Drivers , 2009, Journal of neurogenetics.

[44]  Martin J. Mueller,et al.  Genetic Distortion of the Balance between Punishment and Relief Learning in Drosophila , 2009, Journal of neurogenetics.

[45]  B. Gerber,et al.  ‘Pain relief’ learning in fruit flies , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[46]  Julian Mintseris,et al.  Phosphoproteome analysis of Drosophila melanogaster embryos. , 2008, Journal of proteome research.

[47]  Andreas S. Thum,et al.  Multiple Memory Traces for Olfactory Reward Learning in Drosophila , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  K. Han,et al.  D1 Dopamine Receptor dDA1 Is Required in the Mushroom Body Neurons for Aversive and Appetitive Learning in Drosophila , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  S. Waddell,et al.  Drosophila olfactory memory: single genes to complex neural circuits , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[50]  R. Stocker,et al.  Complex behavioural changes after odour exposure in Drosophila larvae , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[51]  T. Zars,et al.  The conserved protein kinase-A target motif in synapsin of Drosophila is effectively modified by pre-mRNA editing , 2006, BMC Neuroscience.

[52]  G. Nagel,et al.  Light-Induced Activation of Distinct Modulatory Neurons Triggers Appetitive or Aversive Learning in Drosophila Larvae , 2006, Current Biology.

[53]  A. Fiala,et al.  Punishment Prediction by Dopaminergic Neurons in Drosophila , 2005, Current Biology.

[54]  M. Cobb,et al.  Olfactory coding in Drosophila larvae investigated by cross-adaptation , 2005, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[55]  J. Dubnau,et al.  Deconstructing Memory in Drosophila , 2005, Current Biology.

[56]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  Olfactory memory formation in Drosophila: from molecular to systems neuroscience. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[57]  E. Buchner,et al.  A role for Synapsin in associative learning: the Drosophila larva as a study case. , 2005, Learning & memory.

[58]  P. Greengard,et al.  Different Presynaptic Roles of Synapsins at Excitatory and Inhibitory Synapses , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[59]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  An engram found? Evaluating the evidence from fruit flies , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[60]  Hiromu Tanimoto,et al.  Experimental psychology: Event timing turns punishment to reward , 2004, Nature.

[61]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Flies lacking all synapsins are unexpectedly healthy but are impaired in complex behaviour , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[62]  T. Sūdhof The synaptic vesicle cycle. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[63]  N. Blom,et al.  Prediction of post‐translational glycosylation and phosphorylation of proteins from the amino acid sequence , 2004, Proteomics.

[64]  C. Leibold,et al.  Structure–function analysis of the cysteine string protein in Drosophila: cysteine string, linker and C terminus , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[65]  J. Goodship,et al.  Identification of a mutation in synapsin I, a synaptic vesicle protein, in a family with epilepsy , 2004, Journal of Medical Genetics.

[66]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Dopamine and Octopamine Differentiate between Aversive and Appetitive Olfactory Memories in Drosophila , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  M. Heisenberg Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[68]  T. Godenschwege,et al.  Compartmentalization of Central Neurons inDrosophila: A New Strategy of Mosaic Analysis Reveals Localization of Presynaptic Sites to Specific Segments of Neurites , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Localization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. , 2000, Science.

[70]  S. Ueno,et al.  Polymorphisms of the human homologue of the Drosophila white gene are associated with mood and panic disorders , 1999, Molecular Psychiatry.

[71]  P. Greengard,et al.  Synapsins as regulators of neurotransmitter release. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[72]  T. Préat,et al.  Decreased Odor Avoidance after Electric Shock inDrosophila Mutants Biases Learning and Memory Tests , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[73]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  The organization of extrinsic neurons and their implications in the functional roles of the mushroom bodies in Drosophila melanogaster Meigen. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[74]  Paul W. Frankland,et al.  Impaired learning in mice with abnormal short-lived plasticity , 1996, Current Biology.

[75]  T. Godenschwege,et al.  Invertebrate Synapsins: A Single Gene Codes for Several Isoforms in Drosophila , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[76]  R. Schulz,et al.  Expression of the D-MEF2 transcription in the Drosophila brain suggests a role in neuronal cell differentiation. , 1996, Oncogene.

[77]  C. Goodman,et al.  Ectopic and increased expression of fasciclin II alters motoneuron growth cone guidance , 1994, Neuron.

[78]  S. Benzer,et al.  Paralysis and early death in cysteine string protein mutants of Drosophila. , 1994, Science.

[79]  G. Gloor,et al.  Type I repressors of P element mobility. , 1993, Genetics.

[80]  W. Quinn,et al.  Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutantDrosophila melanogaster , 1985, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[81]  J. Kyhse-Andersen Electroblotting of multiple gels: a simple apparatus without buffer tank for rapid transfer of proteins from polyacrylamide to nitrocellulose. , 1984, Journal of biochemical and biophysical methods.

[82]  Y. Jan,et al.  dunce, a mutant of Drosophila deficient in learning. , 1976, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[83]  C. Heth Simultaneous and backward fear conditioning as a function of number of CS-UCS pairings. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[84]  H. Plotkin,et al.  Backward conditioning in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[85]  W. Harris,et al.  Conditioned behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. , 1974, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[86]  R. Solomon,et al.  An opponent-process theory of motivation. I. Temporal dynamics of affect. , 1974, Psychological review.

[87]  V. Lolordo,et al.  Role of safety in the Pavlovian backward fear conditioning procedure. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[88]  Troy Zars,et al.  Learning and memory in Drosophila: behavior, genetics, and neural systems. , 2011, International review of neurobiology.

[89]  Bertram Gerber,et al.  The Drosophila larva as a model for studying chemosensation and chemosensory learning: a review. , 2007, Chemical senses.

[90]  M. Hammer,et al.  Backward inhibitory learning in honeybees: a behavioral analysis of reinforcement processing. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[91]  E. Buchner,et al.  A cysteine-string protein is expressed in retina and brain of Drosophila. , 1990, Journal of neurogenetics.

[92]  R. Solomon,et al.  An Opponent-Process Theory of Motivation , 1978 .

[93]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  Temporal Requirements of the Fragile X Mental Retardation Protein in Modulating Circadian Clock Circuit Synaptic Architecture , 2009, Front. Neural Circuits.