Social status modulates prosocial behavior and egalitarianism in preschool children and adults

Significance Even though humans are the most altruistic species, disparities in prosocial orientation are common and occur across social groups that vary in education, sex roles, biology, and financial resources. In the present research, using different manipulations of social status—defined as the level of social prestige and reputation enjoyed by individuals in the eyes of others—we show that mere incidental low status triggers a prosocial orientation manifested in helping behavior, signaling communal intent, and the endorsement of egalitarian goals and values. These effects start to appear early in human ontogeny. The findings suggest that humans have basic cognitive and motivational programs that they use flexibly as they navigate unstable hierarchies typical in human societies. Humans are a cooperative species, capable of altruism and the creation of shared norms that ensure fairness in society. However, individuals with different educational, cultural, economic, or ethnic backgrounds differ in their levels of social investment and endorsement of egalitarian values. We present four experiments showing that subtle cues to social status (i.e., prestige and reputation in the eyes of others) modulate prosocial orientation. The experiments found that individuals who experienced low status showed more communal and prosocial behavior, and endorsed more egalitarian life goals and values compared with those who experienced high status. Behavioral differences across high- and low-status positions appeared early in human ontogeny (4–5 y of age).

[1]  Anna K. Döring,et al.  Assessing Values at an Early Age: The Picture-Based Value Survey for Children (PBVS–C) , 2010, Journal of personality assessment.

[2]  Joey T. Cheng,et al.  The Impact of Wealth on Prestige and Dominance Rank Relationships , 2013 .

[3]  Allard R. Feddes,et al.  “My In-group is Superior!”: Susceptibility for Radical Right-wing Attitudes and Behaviors in Dutch Youth , 2012 .

[4]  C. Kalish Becoming status conscious , 2005 .

[5]  Jon K. Maner,et al.  Dynamical Evolutionary Psychology: Mapping the Domains of the New Interactionist Paradigm , 2002 .

[6]  Susan T. Fiske,et al.  Control, Interdependence and Power: Understanding Social Cognition in Its Social Context , 1996 .

[7]  C. Zink,et al.  Know Your Place: Neural Processing of Social Hierarchy in Humans , 2008, Neuron.

[8]  A. Bardi,et al.  Value stability and change during self-chosen life transitions: self-selection versus socialization effects. , 2014, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  Amots Zehavi,et al.  The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwin's Puzzle , 1997 .

[10]  S. Schwartz,et al.  Sex differences in value priorities: cross-cultural and multimethod studies. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  H. Triandis Culture and Social Behavior , 2019, Cross-Cultural Explorations.

[12]  Jessica C. Flack,et al.  The emergence of simple and complex power structures through social niche construction. , 2010 .

[13]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Signalers and receivers in animal communication. , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[14]  Mark T. Greenberg,et al.  The Contribution of Inhibitory Control to Preschoolers' Social-Emotional Competence. , 2009 .

[15]  H. Gerard,et al.  Distinctiveness of social categorization and attitude toward ingroup members. , 1974 .

[16]  S. Quartz,et al.  Implicit signals in small group settings and their impact on the expression of cognitive capacity and associated brain responses , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  Jeanne Altmann,et al.  Social Bonds of Female Baboons Enhance Infant Survival , 2003, Science.

[18]  R. Sapolsky The Influence of Social Hierarchy on Primate Health , 2005, Science.

[19]  R. Sapolsky Social Status and Health in Humans and Other Animals , 2004 .

[20]  S. Fiske,et al.  An ambivalent alliance. Hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. , 2001, The American psychologist.

[21]  K. Laland,et al.  Niche construction, human behavior, and the adaptive‐lag hypothesis , 2006 .

[22]  Michael W. Kraus,et al.  Having less, giving more: the influence of social class on prosocial behavior. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  Jim Blascovich,et al.  Challenge and threat responses during downward and upward social comparisons , 2001 .

[24]  Michael Johns,et al.  Stereotype threat and executive resource depletion: examining the influence of emotion regulation. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[25]  S. Schwartz Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries , 1992 .

[26]  Joe C Magee,et al.  From power to action. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees , 2011, Nature.

[28]  R. Byrne,et al.  Machiavellian intelligence : social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans , 1990 .

[29]  Erika Hoff,et al.  Socioeconomic Status and Parenting , 2019, Handbook of Parenting.

[30]  James E. King,et al.  Evolutionary Personality Psychology , 2015, Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences.

[31]  A. Dijksterhuis Think different: the merits of unconscious thought in preference development and decision making. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  J. Spencer An Ambivalent Alliance , 1994 .

[33]  Ana Guinote,et al.  Power and Goal Pursuit , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[34]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  Naomi Ellemers,et al.  Self-Stereotyping in the Face of Threats to Group Status and Distinctiveness: The Role of Group Identification , 1997 .

[36]  S. Schwartz Value orientations: Measurement, antecedents and consequences across nations. , 2007 .

[37]  Jean Decety,et al.  The Neurodevelopment of Empathy in Humans , 2010, Developmental Neuroscience.

[38]  Michael W. Kraus,et al.  Social Class, Contextualism, and Empathic Accuracy , 2010, Psychological science.

[39]  B. Rogers,et al.  Social capital is associated with decreased risk of hunger. , 2004, Social science & medicine.

[40]  C. Shively,et al.  Social status and coronary artery atherosclerosis in female monkeys. , 1994, Arteriosclerosis and thrombosis : a journal of vascular biology.

[41]  J. L. Carroll,et al.  The relation between nonverbal indices of empathy and preschoolers' prosocial behavior , 1986 .

[42]  B. Roberts,et al.  Goal and personality trait development in emerging adulthood. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. , 2001, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[44]  C. Anderson,et al.  Why do dominant personalities attain influence in face-to-face groups? The competence-signaling effects of trait dominance. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  A. Baddeley Exploring the Central Executive , 1996 .

[46]  C. Judd,et al.  Effects of power on perceived and objective group variability: evidence that more powerful groups are more variable. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[47]  L. Cosmides,et al.  Evolutionary psychology: new perspectives on cognition and motivation. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.

[48]  C. Steele,et al.  Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  R. Trivers The Evolution of Reciprocal Altruism , 1971, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[50]  J. Dovidio,et al.  The effects of race, status, and ability on helping behavior. , 1981 .

[51]  J. Silk Social Components of Fitness in Primate Groups , 2007, Science.

[52]  A. Guinote The perception of group variability in a non-minority and a minority context: when adaptation leads to out-group differentiation. , 2001, The British journal of social psychology.

[53]  P. Uhlenberg,et al.  Minority Group Status and Fertility , 1969, American Journal of Sociology.

[54]  C. Kalish BECOMING STATUS CONSCIOUS Children ’ s Appreciation of Social Reality , 2005 .

[55]  V. Skirbekk Fertility trends by social status , 2008 .

[56]  J. Henrich,et al.  The development of contingent reciprocity in children , 2013 .

[57]  E. Paluck,et al.  The contingent smile: a meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.