P300 and individual differences: morning/evening activity preference, food, and time-of-day.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] The effects of time of day and subjects' test expectations on recall and recognition of prose materials , 1989 .
[2] H. Begleiter,et al. Event-related brain potentials in boys at risk for alcoholism. , 1984, Science.
[3] M. J. Blake,et al. Time of day effects on performance in a range of tasks , 1967 .
[4] P. Badia,et al. Habituation of P300 to target stimuli , 1989, Physiology & Behavior.
[5] Carlla S. Smith,et al. Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. , 1989, The Journal of applied psychology.
[6] J. Polich,et al. P300 and time of day: Circadian rhythms, food intake, and body temperature , 1990, Biological Psychology.
[7] W. Pritchard. P300 and EPQ/STPI personality traits☆ , 1989 .
[8] G. Kerkhof,et al. Event-related potentials and auditory signal detection: their diurnal variation for morning-type and evening-type subjects. , 1982, Psychophysiology.
[9] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[10] J. Polich,et al. Normal variation of P300 in children: age, memory span, and head size. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[11] C. P. Browman,et al. Diurnal Periodicity of the Human Vertex Potential , 1980, Perceptual and motor skills.
[12] J. H. Daruna,et al. Introversion, attention and the late positive component of event-related potentials , 1985, Biological Psychology.
[13] T. Monk,et al. Interactions between inter-individual and inter-task differences in the diurnal variation of human performance. , 1986, Chronobiology international.
[14] A. Pettitt,et al. Ciradian performance differences between morning and evening "types". , 1980, Ergonomics.
[15] R. Broughton,et al. Complex event-related potentials (P300 and CNV) and MSLT in the assessment of excessive daytime sleepiness in narcolepsy-cataplexy. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[16] Timothy H. Monk,et al. Circadian rhythms in human memory , 1980 .
[17] J. Richard Jennings,et al. Editorial Policy on Analyses of Variance With Repeated Measures , 1987 .
[18] John Polich,et al. Response mode and P300 from auditory stimuli , 1987, Biological Psychology.
[19] Torbjörn Åkerstedt,et al. Interindividual differences in circadian patterns of catecholamine excretion, body temperature, performance, and subjective arousal , 1976, Biological Psychology.
[20] Pietro Badia,et al. Time of day, repeated testing, and interblock interval effects on P300 amplitude , 1990, Physiology & Behavior.
[21] A. Craig,et al. Speed-accuracy trade-off and time of day. , 1985, Acta psychologica.
[22] S. Steinhauer,et al. Event-related potentials as markers for alcoholism risk in high density families. , 1988, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.
[23] C. P. Browman. Diurnal variation of the human auditory evoked potential , 1979 .
[24] J. Polich,et al. P300 and introverted/extraverted personality types. , 1991, Psychophysiology.
[25] N. Kleitman,et al. Body temperature and performance under different routines. , 1950, Journal of applied physiology.
[26] T. Åkerstedt,et al. Sleepiness on the job: continuously measured EEG changes in train drivers. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[27] G. Heninger,et al. Diurnal variations in the cerebral evoked response and EEG. Relations to 17-hydroxycorticosteroid levels. , 1969, Archives of neurology.
[28] G. Kerkhof. Interindividual differences in the diurnal variation of the human auditory evoked potential , 1980 .
[29] J. Polich,et al. Attention, probability, and task demands as determinants of P300 latency from auditory stimuli. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[30] P. Venables,et al. Enhanced P3 evoked potentials and longer P3 recovery times in psychopaths. , 1988, Psychophysiology.
[31] L Torsvall,et al. Sleepiness in shiftwork. A review with emphasis on continuous monitoring of EEG and EOG. , 1987, Chronobiology international.
[32] J. Horne,et al. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. , 1976, International journal of chronobiology.
[33] John Polich,et al. P300 seasonal variation , 1991, Biological Psychology.
[34] K. Polonsky,et al. Hypoglycmic Thresholds for Cognitive Dysfunction in Humans , 1990, Diabetes.
[35] R Broughton,et al. A comparison of multiple and single sleep latency and cerebral evoked potential (P300) measures in the assessment of excessive daytime sleepiness in narcolepsy-cataplexy. , 1988, Sleep.
[36] Ray Johnson. For Distinguished Early Career Contribution to Psychophysiology: Award Address, 1985 , 1986 .
[37] J. Fröberg. Twenty-four-hour patterns in human performance, subjective and physiological variables and differences between morning and evening active subjects , 1977, Biological Psychology.
[38] V. Gallai†,et al. A study of the P300 component during minor hypoglycaemia. , 1988, Acta neurologica.
[39] John Polich,et al. P300 from identical twins , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[40] T. Allison,et al. Mild Hypoglycemia and Impairment of Brain Stem and Cortical Evoked Potentials in Healthy Subjects , 1990, Diabetes.
[41] T Kobayashi,et al. Short and long ultradian EEG components in daytime arousal. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[42] M. Buchsbaum,et al. Naloxone decreases diurnal variation in pain sensitivity and somatosensory evoked potentials. , 1978 .
[43] G. Kerkhof,et al. Diurnal differences between morning-type and evening-type subjects in self-rated alertness, body temperature and the visual and auditory evoked potential , 1980, Neuroscience Letters.
[44] W. Pritchard,et al. Cognitive event-related potential correlates of schizophrenia. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.
[45] Philippa H. Gander,et al. Task variables determine which biological clock controls circadian rhythms in human performance , 1983, Nature.
[46] F. Bloom,et al. Event-related brain potentials in individuals at high and low risk for developing alcoholism: failure to replicate. , 1988, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.
[47] J. Polich,et al. P300, food consumption, and memory performance. , 1992, Psychophysiology.