Interference Between Accustomed Number–Space Mappings and Unacquainted Letter–Space Mappings in a Button Press Task
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. Dehaene,et al. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[2] Jamie I. D. Campbell. Handbook of mathematical cognition , 2004 .
[3] Michael D. Dodd,et al. Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[4] Motonori Yamaguchi,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility with pure and mixed mappings in a flight task environment. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[5] Wim Fias,et al. Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect. , 2006, Acta psychologica.
[6] R. Proctor,et al. Mixing compatible and incompatible mappings: Elimination, reduction, and enhancement of spatial compatibility effects , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[7] Wim Fias,et al. The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized , 2003, Cognition.
[8] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[9] Andrea M Philipp,et al. Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.
[10] Wim Fias,et al. Spatial representation of numbers. , 2004 .
[11] Giovanni Galfano,et al. Number magnitude orients attention, but not against one’s will , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[12] D. Meyer,et al. Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[14] D. A. Grant,et al. Learning and performance on a key-pressing task as function of the degree of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1955, Journal of experimental psychology.
[15] Claudia Bauzer Medeiros,et al. A Direct Manipulation User Interface for Querying Geographic Databases , 1995 .
[16] R. Proctor,et al. Determinants of the benefit for consistent spatial mappings in dual-task performance of three-choice tasks , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[17] W. Glaser,et al. Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[18] R. Proctor,et al. Age differences in response selection for pure and mixed stimulus-response mappings and tasks. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[19] R. Kliegl,et al. Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[20] Robert W Proctor,et al. Determinants of the benefit for consistent stimulus-response mappings in dual-task performance of four-choice tasks , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[21] D. Woltz,et al. Negative transfer errors in sequential cognitive skills: strong-but-wrong sequence application. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[22] J. Ridley. Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .
[23] Sean P. Gyll,et al. The role of attention processes in near transfer of cognitive skills , 2000 .
[24] H. Bekkering,et al. Short Article: Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements Influence Spatial–Numerical Associations , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[25] Michael K. Gardner,et al. Individual differences in undetected errors in skilled cognitive performance , 1997 .
[26] R. D. Gordon,et al. Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.
[27] S. Greenberg,et al. The Psychology of Everyday Things , 2012 .
[28] Cleotilde Gonzalez,et al. Dissociation of S-R compatibility and Simon effects with mixed tasks and mappings. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] Patrik Vuilleumier,et al. The Number Space and Neglect , 2004, Cortex.
[30] R. Kliegl,et al. Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[31] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[32] Ritske de Jong,et al. Strategical determinants of compatibility effects with task uncertainty , 1995 .
[33] F. Verbruggen,et al. Increasing the difficulty of response selection does not increase the switch cost. , 2009, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[34] Christopher Hamblin,et al. Design of a Cursor Control Device for the Orion Crew Exploration Vehicle , 2008 .
[35] Peter Brugger,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[36] M. H. Fischer,et al. The Future for Snarc Could Be Stark… , 2006, Cortex.
[37] R. Proctor,et al. Influence of bias on visuospatial and verbal compatibility effects in mixed-mapping environments , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[38] R. L. Deininger,et al. S-R compatibility: correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[39] Shaffer Lh. CHOICE REACTION WITH VARIABLE S-R MAPPING. , 1965 .
[40] J. Danckert. Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX Wolfgang Prinz, Bernhard Hommel (Eds.), Oxford University Press, 2002, Price: £ 65.00, ISBN: 0-19-851069 , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[41] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[42] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[43] L. Shaffer. CHOICE REACTION WITH VARIABLE S-R MAPPING. , 1965, Journal of experimental psychology.
[44] R. Proctor,et al. Stimulus-Response Compatibility Principles: Data, Theory, and Application , 2006 .
[45] Spatial S-R compatibility: positional instruction vs. compatibility instruction. , 1994, Acta psychologica.
[46] W Fias,et al. Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[47] Takeshi Hatta,et al. Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect , 2004, Memory & cognition.
[48] A. Kingstone,et al. The number line effect reflects top-down control , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[49] J. R. Simon. The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .
[50] John E. Laird,et al. Stimulus-Response Compatibility , 1986 .
[51] R. Proctor,et al. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[52] Samar Zebian,et al. Linkages between Number Concepts, Spatial Thinking, and Directionality of Writing: The SNARC Effect and the REVERSE SNARC Effect in English and Arabic Monoliterates, Biliterates, and Illiterate Arabic Speakers , 2005 .
[53] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .
[54] Dana Ganor-Stern,et al. Automaticity of two-digit numbers. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[55] Wayne D. Gray,et al. An integrated model of cognitive control in task switching. , 2008, Psychological review.
[56] Patrick C. Kyllonen,et al. Memory for order of operations in the acquisition and transfer of sequential cognitive skills. , 1996 .