Temporal information for spatially constrained locomotion

How is locomotion adapted to spatial environmental constraints? The control of this everyday behavior is claimed to be based on information that specifies either spatial or temporal properties of the actor-environment system. Although studies on open-loop locomotor pointing (i.e., the positioning of a foot on a target on the floor while walking) agree on the use of spatial information, studies on closed-loop locomotor pointing propose the use of temporal information. Here, we test the hypothesis of closed-loop locomotor pointing based on temporal information, by dissociating spatial and temporal information in a virtual reality setup (virtual environment connected to a treadmill). The results support this hypothesis and shed some light on the type of temporal information that is used. The performed dissociation between spatial and temporal information, however, does not rule out a control based on a continuous updating of spatial information. Therefore, our conclusion on the use of temporal information was moderated.

[1]  G. Montagne,et al.  The study of locomotor pointing in virtual reality: The validation of a test set-up , 2000, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[2]  M. Hollands,et al.  Visually guided stepping under conditions of step cycle-related denial of visual information , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  M. Kaiser,et al.  Optical specification of time-to-passage: Observers' sensitivity to global tau. , 1993 .

[4]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.

[5]  M. Laurent,et al.  A Perception-Action Coupling Type of Control in Long Jumping , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[6]  J. Thomson Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  M K Kaiser,et al.  Optical specification of time-to-passage: observers' sensitivity to global tau. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Gentaro Taga,et al.  A model of the neuro-musculo-skeletal system for anticipatory adjustment of human locomotion during obstacle avoidance , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.

[9]  M. Bonnard,et al.  Interaction between different sensory cues in the control of human gait , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  P. Bessou,et al.  Simultaneous recording of longitudinal displacements of both feet during human walking. , 1988, Journal de physiologie.

[11]  H. Sedgwick Environment-Centered Representation of Spatial Layout: Available Visual Information from Texture and Perspective , 1983 .

[12]  Gilles Montagne,et al.  Spatially constrained locomotion under informational conflict , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  James A. Thomson,et al.  On-Line Updating of Spatial Information Druing Locomotion Without Vision. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[14]  A. Patla How Is Human Gait Controlled by Vision , 1998 .

[15]  David N. Lee,et al.  Regulation of gait in long jumping. , 1982 .

[16]  J. Thomson,et al.  The role of visual information in control of a constrained locomotor task. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[17]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  M. Bonnard,et al.  Intentional on-line control of propulsive forces in human gait , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  David N. Lee,et al.  A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.

[20]  J M Loomis,et al.  Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  W. H. Warren,et al.  Visual control of step length during running over irregular terrain. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  M. J. Buekers,et al.  The control of human locomotor pointing under restricted informational conditions , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[23]  J. Gibson Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. , 1998, British journal of psychology.

[24]  V Cavallo,et al.  How is gait visually regulated when the head is travelling faster than the legs? , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[25]  J. Philbeck,et al.  Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[27]  A Berthoz,et al.  Spatial, not temporal cues drive predictive orienting movements during navigation: a virtual reality study , 2000, Neuroreport.

[28]  William H. Warren,et al.  Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.