Effects of aging on value-directed modulation of semantic network activity during verbal learning

While impairments in memory recall are apparent in aging, older adults show a remarkably preserved ability to selectively remember information deemed valuable. Here, we use fMRI to compare brain activation in healthy older and younger adults during encoding of high and low value words to determine whether there are differences in how older adults achieve value-directed memory selectivity. We find that memory selectivity in older adults is associated with value-related changes in activation during word presentation in left hemisphere regions that are involved in semantic processing, similar to young adults. However, highly selective young adults show a relatively greater increase in semantic network activity during encoding of high-value items, whereas highly selective older adults show relatively diminished activity during encoding of low-value items. Additionally, only younger adults showed value-related increases in activity in semantic and reward processing regions during presentation of the value cue preceding each to-be-remembered word. Young adults therefore respond to cue value more proactively than do older adults, yet the magnitude of value-related differences in cue period brain activity did not predict individual differences in memory selectivity. Thus, our data also show that age-related reductions in prestimulus activity do not always lead to inefficient performance.

[1]  T. Braver,et al.  Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. , 2007 .

[2]  E. Auerbach,et al.  Relative Shift in Activity from Medial to Lateral Frontal Cortex During Internally Versus Externally Guided Word Generation , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Alan D. Castel,et al.  The Adaptive and Strategic Use of Memory By Older Adults: Evaluative Processing and Value-Directed Remembering , 2007 .

[4]  Shinji Kitagami,et al.  Consolidation power of extrinsic rewards: reward cues enhance long-term memory for irrelevant past events. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  An expectation-based memory deficit in aging , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  N. Alpert,et al.  Prefrontal regions supporting spontaneous and directed application of verbal learning strategies: evidence from PET. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  S. Sikström,et al.  Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  M. Rugg,et al.  Brain activity before an event predicts later recollection , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Donna Rose Addis,et al.  Prefrontal and hippocampal contributions to the generation and binding of semantic associations during successful encoding , 2006, NeuroImage.

[10]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Correlates of Individual Differences in Memory , 2006, Neuron.

[11]  Gary H. Glover,et al.  Variable effects of aging on frontal lobe contributions to memory , 2002, Neuroreport.

[12]  G. Galli What Makes Deeply Encoded Items Memorable? Insights into the Levels of Processing Framework from Neuroimaging and Neuromodulation , 2014, Front. Psychiatry.

[13]  W. Revelle psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research , 2017 .

[14]  Michael J. Martinez,et al.  Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM‐152 brain template , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[15]  R. Knight,et al.  Age-related top-down suppression deficit in the early stages of cortical visual memory processing , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  M. Rugg,et al.  Prestimulus hippocampal activity predicts later recollection , 2009, Hippocampus.

[17]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Group comparisons: imaging the aging brain. , 2008, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[18]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Aging Gracefully: Compensatory Brain Activity in High-Performing Older Adults , 2002, NeuroImage.

[19]  Michel Thiebaut de Schotten,et al.  Short frontal lobe connections of the human brain , 2012, Cortex.

[20]  Norbou Buchler,et al.  Where is ELSA? The early to late shift in aging. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  Aaron S Benjamin,et al.  The effects of aging on selectivity and control in short-term recall , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[22]  S. M. Daselaar,et al.  When less means more: deactivations during encoding that predict subsequent memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[23]  K. Paller,et al.  Observing the transformation of experience into memory , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  Marco Catani,et al.  Brain Mapping: An encyclopedic reference , 2015 .

[25]  Keith D. White,et al.  Structural connectivity of Broca's area and medial frontal cortex , 2010, NeuroImage.

[26]  Alison R. Preston,et al.  Reward Modulation of Hippocampal Subfield Activation during Successful Associative Encoding and Retrieval , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  David C. Van Essen,et al.  Application of Information Technology: An Integrated Software Suite for Surface-based Analyses of Cerebral Cortex , 2001, J. Am. Medical Informatics Assoc..

[28]  Hannah S. Locke,et al.  Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human prefrontal cortex , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[29]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Region-specific changes in prefrontal function with age: a review of PET and fMRI studies on working and episodic memory. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  J. H. Steiger Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. , 1980 .

[31]  D. Balota,et al.  Memory efficiency and the strategic control of attention at encoding: impairments of value-directed remembering in Alzheimer's disease. , 2009, Neuropsychology.

[32]  James Z. Chadick,et al.  Structural and functional differences in medial prefrontal cortex underlies distractibility and suppression deficits in aging , 2014, Nature Communications.

[33]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[34]  R. Cabeza Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[35]  Eyes wide open: enhanced pupil dilation when selectively studying important information , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  K. Murayama,et al.  Money enhances memory consolidation – But only for boring material , 2011, Cognition.

[37]  Edson Amaro,et al.  Bilateral activation of the prefrontal cortex after strategic semantic cognitive training , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[38]  M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al.  The Neural Organization of Semantic Control: TMS Evidence for a Distributed Network in Left Inferior Frontal and Posterior Middle Temporal Gyrus , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  Barbara J Knowlton,et al.  Value-based modulation of memory encoding involves strategic engagement of fronto-temporal semantic processing regions , 2014, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[40]  Jelliffe Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde , 1910 .

[41]  M. Rugg,et al.  When more means less neural activity related to unsuccessful memory encoding , 2001, Current Biology.

[42]  Andrew J. Kelly,et al.  Working memory and the strategic control of attention in older and younger adults. , 2013, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[43]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Recovering Meaning Left Prefrontal Cortex Guides Controlled Semantic Retrieval , 2001, Neuron.

[44]  M. Catani,et al.  A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  W. K. Cullen,et al.  Dopamine-dependent facilitation of LTP induction in hippocampal CA1 by exposure to spatial novelty , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  Michael P Toglia Withstanding the test of time: The 1978 semantic word norms , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[47]  W. F. Battig,et al.  Handbook of semantic word norms , 1978 .

[48]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Expectation-Driven Changes in Cortical Functional Connectivity Influence Working Memory and Long-Term Memory Performance , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  Nico Bunzeck,et al.  Contextual interaction between novelty and reward processing within the mesolimbic system , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[51]  Rutvik H. Desai,et al.  The neurobiology of semantic memory , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  D. Shohamy,et al.  Dopamine and adaptive memory , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[53]  J. Cerella,et al.  Aging, executive control, and attention: a review of meta-analyses , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[54]  Tracy H. Wang,et al.  Effects of age on negative subsequent memory effects associated with the encoding of item and item-context information. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  Matthias J. Gruber,et al.  States of Curiosity Modulate Hippocampus-Dependent Learning via the Dopaminergic Circuit , 2014, Neuron.

[56]  J. Logan,et al.  Under-Recruitment and Nonselective Recruitment Dissociable Neural Mechanisms Associated with Aging , 2002, Neuron.

[57]  M. Rugg,et al.  The relationship between aging, performance, and the neural correlates of successful memory encoding. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[58]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Fast robust automated brain extraction , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[59]  Theodore P. Zanto,et al.  Top-Down Suppression , 2015 .

[60]  Jeffrey W. Cooney,et al.  Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[61]  M. Guitart-Masip,et al.  NOvelty-related Motivation of Anticipation and exploration by Dopamine (NOMAD): Implications for healthy aging , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[62]  James K. Nelson,et al.  Age Differences in Deactivation: A Link to Cognitive Control? , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  S. Holm A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure , 1979 .

[64]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Reward-Motivated Learning: Mesolimbic Activation Precedes Memory Formation , 2006, Neuron.

[65]  Vincent L. Gracco,et al.  Contribution of the frontal lobe to externally and internally specified verbal responses: fMRI evidence , 2006, NeuroImage.

[66]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Cognitive and neural effects of semantic encoding strategy training in older adults. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[67]  A. Dale,et al.  Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. , 1998, Science.

[68]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data , 2011, Nature Methods.

[69]  Richard J. Addante,et al.  Pre-stimulus neural activity predicts successful encoding of inter-item associations , 2015, NeuroImage.

[70]  Vinod Venkatraman,et al.  A Parallel Functional Topography between Medial and Lateral Prefrontal Cortex: Evidence and Implications for Cognitive Control , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[71]  BRIEF REPORT Betting on Memory Leads to Metacognitive Improvement by Younger and Older Adults , 2011 .

[72]  Holly J. Bowen,et al.  Reward-enhanced memory in younger and older adults. , 2014, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[73]  Michael Brady,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[74]  C. Savage,et al.  Brain regions supporting verbal memory improvement in healthy older subjects. , 2014, Arquivos de neuro-psiquiatria.

[75]  Claudio Babiloni,et al.  Age-Related Functional Changes of Prefrontal Cortex in Long-Term Memory: A Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[76]  Matthias J. Gruber,et al.  Voluntary Control over Prestimulus Activity Related to Encoding , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[77]  T. Braver,et al.  Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and prefrontal function in healthy aging. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[78]  Timothy S. Coalson,et al.  Parcellations and hemispheric asymmetries of human cerebral cortex analyzed on surface-based atlases. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[79]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[80]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[81]  Alan D Castel,et al.  Memory for general and specific value information in younger and older adults: Measuring the limits of strategic control , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[82]  Ronald Hübner,et al.  Does attentional selectivity in global/local processing improve discretely or gradually? , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[83]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Mesolimbic novelty processing in older adults. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[84]  T. Braver The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[85]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Temporal Autocorrelation in Univariate Linear Modeling of FMRI Data , 2001, NeuroImage.

[86]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.