The auditory motion aftereffect: its tuning and specificity in the spatial and frequency domains.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Louis Gates. The After-Effect of Visually Observed Movement , 1934 .
[2] J. Gaddum. Probit Analysis , 1948, Nature.
[3] H. Holland,et al. The Archimedes Spiral , 1957, Nature.
[4] A. Mills. On the minimum audible angle , 1958 .
[5] H. Barlow,et al. Evidence for a Physiological Explanation of the Waterfall Phenomenon and Figural After-effects , 1963, Nature.
[6] R. Sekuler,et al. Aftereffect of Seen Motion with a Stabilized Retinal Image , 1963, Science.
[7] M. M. Taylor,et al. Tracking the Decay of the After-Effect of Seen Rotary Movement , 1963, Perceptual and motor skills.
[8] A Pantle,et al. A model for after-effects of seen movement. , 1967, Vision research.
[9] A. Pantle,et al. Velocity-sensitive elements in human vision: initial psychophysical evidence. , 1968, Vision research.
[10] E. Shaw,et al. Sound pressure generated in an external-ear replica and real human ears by a nearby point source. , 1968, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[11] D. Regan,et al. Evidence for the existence of neural mechanisms selectively sensitive to the direction of movement in space , 1973, The Journal of physiology.
[12] D. Regan,et al. Disparity Detectors in Human Depth Perception: Evidence for Directional Selectivity , 1973, Science.
[13] R Over,et al. Spatial determinants of the aftereffect of seen motion. , 1973, Vision research.
[14] R. M. Sachs,et al. Anthropometric manikin for acoustic research. , 1975, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[15] J S Lappin,et al. On the relation between time and space in the visual discrimination of velocity. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] M. Cynader,et al. Stereoscopic subsystems for position in depth and for motion in depth , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[17] D. Grantham,et al. Auditory motion aftereffects , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.
[18] R. P. O'Shea,et al. Interocular transfer of the motion after-effect is not reduced by binocular rivalry , 1981, Vision Research.
[19] Spatial Frequency Selectivity of Spiral Aftereffect , 1982, Perceptual and motor skills.
[20] N. Mai,et al. Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[21] W. L. Brigner. Is Velocity of Motion Aftereffect Proportional to Velocity of Induction , 1986, Perceptual and motor skills.
[22] D W Grantham,et al. Detection and discrimination of simulated motion of auditory targets in the horizontal plane. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[23] L. Rosenblum,et al. Relative Effectiveness of Three Stimulus Variables for Locating a Moving Sound Source , 1987, Perception.
[24] M. Cynader,et al. Direction-selective adaptation in simple and complex cells in cat striate cortex. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.
[25] D. Grantham. Motion aftereffects with horizontally moving sound sources in the free field , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[26] A. Saul,et al. Adaptation in single units in visual cortex: The tuning of aftereffects in the spatial domain , 1989, Visual Neuroscience.
[27] M. Hershenson. Duration, time constant, and decay of the linear motion aftereffect as a function of inspection duration , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[28] B. Columbia.,et al. Adaptation in single units in visual cortex: The tuning of aftereffects in the temporal domain , 1989 .
[29] D. M. Green,et al. Directional dependence of interaural envelope delays. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[30] Max S. Cynader,et al. Aural intensity for a moving source , 1991, Hearing Research.
[31] D. M. Green,et al. Sound localization by human listeners. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.
[32] Elizabeth M. Wenzel,et al. Localization with non-individualized virtual acoustic display cues , 1991, CHI.
[33] Changing-loudness aftereffect following simulated movement: implications for channel hypotheses concerning sound level change and movement. , 1992, The Journal of general psychology.
[34] C. Baker,et al. Spatial frequency selective mechanisms underlying the motion aftereffect , 1992, Vision Research.
[35] D. Perrott,et al. Minimum Audible Movement Angle as a Function of the Azimuth and Elevation of the Source , 1992, Human factors.
[36] Chandler Dw,et al. Minimum audible movement angle in the horizontal plane as a function of stimulus frequency and bandwidth, source azimuth, and velocity. , 1992 .
[37] N. V. Swindale,et al. Spectral motion produces an auditory after-effect , 1993, Nature.
[38] M. Hershenson. Linear and rotation motion aftereffects as a function of inspection duration , 1993, Vision Research.
[39] M. Cynader,et al. The time course of direction-selective adaptation in simple and complex cells in cat striate cortex. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.
[40] N J Wade,et al. A Selective History of the Study of Visual Motion Aftereffects , 1994, Perception.
[41] W H Ehrenstein,et al. Auditory Aftereffects following Simulated Motion Produced by Varying Interaural Intensity or Time , 1994, Perception.
[42] A. Rees,et al. Evidence for a sound movement area in the human cerebral cortex , 1996, Nature.