Motor Variability Arises from a Slow Random Walk in Neural State

Even well practiced movements cannot be repeated without variability. This variability is thought to reflect “noise” in movement preparation or execution. However, we show that, for both professional baseball pitchers and macaque monkeys making reaching movements, motor variability can be decomposed into two statistical components, a slowly drifting mean and fast trial-by-trial fluctuations about the mean. The preparatory activity of dorsal premotor cortex/primary motor cortex neurons in monkey exhibits similar statistics. Although the neural and behavioral drifts appear to be correlated, neural activity does not account for trial-by-trial fluctuations in movement, which must arise elsewhere, likely downstream. The statistics of this drift are well modeled by a double-exponential autocorrelation function, with time constants similar across the neural and behavioral drifts in two monkeys, as well as the drifts observed in baseball pitching. These time constants can be explained by an error-corrective learning processes and agree with learning rates measured directly in previous experiments. Together, these results suggest that the central contributions to movement variability are not simply trial-by-trial fluctuations but are rather the result of longer-timescale processes that may arise from motor learning.

[1]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Heterogeneous Representations in the Superior Parietal Lobule Are Common across Reaches to Visual and Proprioceptive Targets , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  R. J. Beers,et al.  Motor Learning Is Optimally Tuned to the Properties of Motor Noise , 2009, Neuron.

[3]  Xin Huang,et al.  Noise correlations in cortical area MT and their potential impact on trial-by-trial variation in the direction and speed of smooth-pursuit eye movements. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning from Sensory and Reward Prediction Errors during Motor Adaptation , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[5]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Modeling Sensorimotor Learning with Linear Dynamical Systems , 2006 .

[6]  Marc W Slutzky,et al.  Statistical assessment of the stability of neural movement representations. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Henry Szechtman,et al.  Longlasting consequences of chronic treatment with the dopamine agonist quinpirole for the undrugged behavior of rats , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Motor Learning with Unstable Neural Representations , 2007, Neuron.

[9]  John P. Cunningham,et al.  Single-Neuron Stability during Repeated Reaching in Macaque Premotor Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Calibration of visually guided reaching is driven by error-corrective learning and internal dynamics. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[12]  L. Pinneo On noise in the nervous system. , 1966, Psychological review.

[13]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Random walk of motor planning in task-irrelevant dimensions. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  A. Grinvald,et al.  Dynamics of Ongoing Activity: Explanation of the Large Variability in Evoked Cortical Responses , 1996, Science.

[15]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Variability and Correlated Noise in the Discharge of Neurons in Motor and Parietal Areas of the Primate Cortex , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[17]  D. Sofge THE ROLE OF EXPLORATION IN LEARNING CONTROL , 1992 .

[18]  A. Schwartz,et al.  Recording from the same neurons chronically in motor cortex. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  W. Bialek,et al.  A sensory source for motor variation , 2005, Nature.

[20]  Raymond J. Delnicki,et al.  Overcoming Motor “Forgetting” Through Reinforcement Of Learned Actions , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  K. Shenoy,et al.  A Central Source of Movement Variability , 2006, Neuron.

[22]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  How Each Movement Changes the Next: An Experimental and Theoretical Study of Fast Adaptive Priors in Reaching , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Jim Albert,et al.  Baseball Data at Season, Play-by-Play, and Pitch-by-Pitch Levels , 2010 .

[24]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  R A Normann,et al.  The Utah intracortical Electrode Array: a recording structure for potential brain-computer interfaces. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[26]  Mikhail A Lebedev,et al.  Stable Ensemble Performance with Single-neuron Variability during Reaching Movements in Primates , 2022 .

[27]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: effects of practice, uncertainty, and change in target location. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[29]  A. Riehle,et al.  The predictive value for performance speed of preparatory changes in neuronal activity of the monkey motor and premotor cortex , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[30]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  The intrinsic value of visual information affects saccade velocities , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.