Infants Associate Praise and Admonishment with Fair and Unfair Individuals.

Recent evidence suggests that infants possess a rudimentary sensitivity to fairness: infants expect resources to be distributed fairly and equally, and prefer individuals that distribute resources fairly over those that do so unfairly. The goal of the present work was to determine whether infants' evaluations of fair and unfair individuals also includes an understanding that fair individuals are worthy of praise and unfair individuals are worthy of admonishment. After watching individuals distribute goods fairly or unfairly to recipients, 15-month-old (Experiments 1 and 2) and 13-month-old (Experiment 3) infants took part in a test phase in which they saw only the distributors' faces accompanied by praise or admonishment. Across all experiments, infants differentially shifted their visual attention to images of the fair and unfair distributors as a function of the accompanying praise or admonishment, although the direction in which they did so varied by age. Thus, by the start of the second year of life, infants appear to perceive fair individuals as morally praiseworthy and unfair individuals as morally blameworthy.

[1]  B. Roder,et al.  Infants' Preferences for Familiarity and Novelty During the Course of Visual Processing. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[2]  Luca Surian,et al.  Do infants detect indirect reciprocity? , 2013, Cognition.

[3]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Developmental Differences in Infants' Fairness Expectations From 6 to 15 Months of Age. , 2017, Child development.

[4]  Susan J. Hespos,et al.  Nonhuman primate vocalizations support categorization in very young human infants , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Do Infants Have a Sense of Fairness? , 2012, Psychological science.

[6]  Emmanuel Dupoux,et al.  Friend or Foe? Early Social Evaluation of Human Interactions , 2014, PloS one.

[7]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Fairness Expectations and Altruistic Sharing in 15-Month-Old Human Infants , 2011, PloS one.

[8]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Foundations of cooperation in young children , 2008, Cognition.

[9]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Ten-month-old infants use prior information to identify an actor's goal. , 2009, Developmental science.

[10]  A. Woodward,et al.  Not all emotions are created equal: the negativity bias in social-emotional development. , 2008, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  Gail D. Heyman,et al.  Collaboration promotes proportional reasoning about resource distribution in young children. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[12]  Renee L Baillargeon,et al.  Psychological and Sociomoral Reasoning in Infancy , 2014 .

[13]  Olesya Govorun,et al.  An inkblot for attitudes: affect misattribution as implicit measurement. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  L. Bahrick,et al.  Infants' Bimodal Perception of Gender , 1991 .

[15]  E. Turiel,et al.  The Development of Social Knowledge: Morality and Convention , 1983 .

[16]  J. Hamlin Failed attempts to help and harm: Intention versus outcome in preverbal infants’ social evaluations , 2013, Cognition.

[17]  Satsuki Nakai,et al.  Distinguishing novelty and familiarity effects in infant preference procedures , 2004 .

[18]  J. Hamlin,et al.  Three-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. , 2010, Developmental science.

[19]  M. Deutsch Equity, Equality, and Need: What Determines Which Value Will Be Used as the Basis of Distributive Justice? , 1975 .

[20]  M. Birnbaum,et al.  Compensatory effects in moral judgment: two rights don't make up for a wrong. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  K. Wynn,et al.  OF DISPOSITIONAL STATES BY 12-MONTH-OLDS , 2003 .

[22]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  R. Flom,et al.  Infants' intermodal perception of canine (Canis familairis) facial expressions and vocalizations. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[24]  L. Surian,et al.  Infants Distinguish Antisocial Actions Directed towards Fair and Unfair Agents , 2014, PloS one.

[25]  Madan M. Pillutla,et al.  Unfairness, Anger, and Spite: Emotional Rejections of Ultimatum Offers , 1996 .

[26]  J. Deloache,et al.  When Getting Something Good is Bad: Even Three-year-olds React to Inequality , 2011 .

[27]  S. Carey,et al.  How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  Joshua Knobe,et al.  Acting Intentionally and the Side-Effect Effect , 2006, Psychological science.

[29]  Paul Bloom,et al.  Social Evaluation by Preverbal Infants , 2008, Pediatric Research.

[30]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Three-year-old children intervene in third-party moral transgressions. , 2010, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[31]  A. Premack,et al.  Infants Attribute Value to the Goal-Directed Actions of Self-propelled Objects , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  E. Spelke Perceiving Bimodally Specified Events in Infancy , 1979 .

[33]  J. Hamlin,et al.  Context-dependent social evaluation in 4.5-month-old human infants: the role of domain-general versus domain-specific processes in the development of social evaluation , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[34]  Mahzarin R. Banaji,et al.  The Development of Implicit Attitudes , 2006, Psychological science.

[35]  J. Smetana Preschool children's conceptions of moral and social rules. , 1981 .

[36]  David M. Sanbonmatsu,et al.  On the automatic activation of attitudes. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  J. Sommerville,et al.  “I pick you”: the impact of fairness and race on infants’ selection of social partners , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[38]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children’s understanding of violations of property rights , 2011, Cognition.

[39]  Larry Nucci,et al.  Social interactions and the development of social concepts in preschool children. , 1978 .

[40]  R. Fazio,et al.  Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  J. Sommerville,et al.  The Development of Fairness Expectations and Prosocial Behavior in the Second Year of Life , 2013 .

[42]  J. Fowler,et al.  The Role of Egalitarian Motives in Altruistic Punishment , 2007 .

[43]  E. Spelke,et al.  Haptic perception of objects in infancy , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  Donal E. Carlston,et al.  Caught in the act: When impressions based on highly diagnostic behaviours are resistant to contradiction , 1992 .

[45]  M. Banaji,et al.  Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.

[46]  E. W. Ames,et al.  A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. , 1988 .

[47]  Larry Nucci,et al.  Children's Responses to Moral and Social Conventional Transgressions in Free-Play Settings. , 1982 .

[48]  James H. Fowler,et al.  Egalitarian motives in humans , 2007, Nature.

[49]  L. Surian,et al.  The developmental roots of fairness: infants' reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. , 2011, Developmental science.

[50]  B. Kenward,et al.  Enactment of Third-Party Punishment by 4-Year-Olds , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[51]  S. Carey,et al.  First-person action experience reveals sensitivity to action efficiency in prereaching infants , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.