Dynamic similarity promotes interpersonal coordination in joint action

Human movement has been studied for decades, and dynamic laws of motion that are common to all humans have been derived. Yet, every individual moves differently from everyone else (faster/slower, harder/smoother, etc.). We propose here an index of such variability, namely an individual motor signature (IMS) able to capture the subtle differences in the way each of us moves. We show that the IMS of a person is time-invariant and that it significantly differs from those of other individuals. This allows us to quantify the dynamic similarity, a measure of rapport between dynamics of different individuals' movements, and demonstrate that it facilitates coordination during interaction. We use our measure to confirm a key prediction of the theory of similarity that coordination between two individuals performing a joint-action task is higher if their motions share similar dynamic features. Furthermore, we use a virtual avatar driven by an interactive cognitive architecture based on feedback control theory to explore the effects of different kinematic features of the avatar motion on coordination with human players.

[1]  C. Chatfield Continuous Univariate Distributions, Vol. 1 , 1995 .

[2]  Angelika Foerster,et al.  Coordination Neural Behavioral And Social Dynamics , 2016 .

[3]  B. Manly Multivariate Statistical Methods : A Primer , 1986 .

[4]  Patrick J. F. Groenen,et al.  Modern Multidimensional Scaling: Theory and Applications , 2003 .

[5]  N. L. Johnson,et al.  Continuous Univariate Distributions. , 1995 .

[6]  Michael J Richardson,et al.  Computation of continuous relative phase and modulation of frequency of human movement. , 2011, Journal of biomechanics.

[7]  V. Folkes,et al.  Forming Relationships and the Matching Hypothesis , 1982 .

[8]  Cecilia Heyes,et al.  Self-recognition of avatar motion: how do I know it's me? , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  Chao Zhai,et al.  Adaptive tracking control of a virtual player in the mirror game , 2014, 53rd IEEE Conference on Decision and Control.

[10]  N. Hogan,et al.  On rhythmic and discrete movements: reflections, definitions and implications for motor control , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  M. Muskulus,et al.  Wasserstein distances in the analysis of time series and dynamical systems , 2011 .

[12]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Dynamics of Interpersonal Coordination , 2008 .

[13]  Björn Kralemann,et al.  Phase dynamics of coupled oscillators reconstructed from data. , 2008, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.

[14]  J. Kelso,et al.  The human dynamic clamp as a paradigm for social interaction , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  T. Pyszczynski,et al.  Seeing I to I: a pathway to interpersonal connectedness. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  J. Kelso,et al.  Social coordination dynamics: Measuring human bonding , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[18]  L. Guibas,et al.  The Earth Mover''s Distance: Lower Bounds and Invariance under Translation , 1997 .

[19]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Bodily synchronization underlying joke telling , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[20]  S. McEachern,et al.  The need for an integrated theoretical framework for researching the influence of group diversity on performance , 2009 .

[21]  Y. Paulignan,et al.  An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.

[22]  Johann Issartel,et al.  The relevance of the cross-wavelet transform in the analysis of human interaction – a tutorial , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[23]  Rosa Sullivan,et al.  The Mirror Game , 1974 .

[24]  U. Alon,et al.  Individuality and Togetherness in Joint Improvised Motion , 2014, PloS one.

[25]  J. Guckenheimer,et al.  Estimating the phase of synchronized oscillators. , 2008, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.

[26]  B G Bardy,et al.  Level of gymnastic skill as an intrinsic constraint on postural coordination. , 1999, Journal of sports sciences.

[27]  Nicholas I. Fisher,et al.  Statistical Analysis of Circular Data , 1993 .

[28]  Chao Zhai,et al.  A novel cognitive architecture for a human-like virtual player in the mirror game , 2014, 2014 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC).

[29]  Aslak Grinsted,et al.  Nonlinear Processes in Geophysics Application of the Cross Wavelet Transform and Wavelet Coherence to Geophysical Time Series , 2022 .

[30]  V K Jirsa,et al.  Recruitment of degrees of freedom stabilizes coordination. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Moving attractive virtual agent improves interpersonal coordination stability. , 2015, Human movement science.

[32]  J. Kelso,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992 .

[33]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Social Connection Through Joint Action and Interpersonal Coordination , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[34]  T. Flash,et al.  Minimum-jerk, two-thirds power law, and isochrony: converging approaches to movement planning. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  Armin Fuchs,et al.  Beyond the blank slate: routes to learning new coordination patterns depend on the intrinsic dynamics of the learner—experimental evidence and theoretical model , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[36]  J. Shohat,et al.  INEQUALITIES FOR MOMENTS OF FREQUENCY FUNCTIONS AND FOR VARIOUS STATISTICAL CONSTANTS , 1929 .

[37]  U. Alon,et al.  The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics of two people improvising motion together , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Peter J. Bickel,et al.  The Earth Mover's distance is the Mallows distance: some insights from statistics , 2001, Proceedings Eighth IEEE International Conference on Computer Vision. ICCV 2001.

[39]  D. Lakens,et al.  If They Move in Sync, They Must Feel in Sync: Movement Synchrony Leads to Attributions of Rapport and Entitativity , 2011 .

[40]  Philip W. Fink,et al.  Recruitment of degrees of freedom stabilizes coordination. , 2000 .

[41]  J. Krause,et al.  The role of individuality in collective group movement , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  I. Chakravarti,et al.  Handbook of Methods of Applied Statistics:@@@Volume I: Techniques of Computation, Descriptive Methods, and Statistical Inference@@@Volume II: Planning of Surveys and Experiments. , 1968 .

[43]  B. P. Murphy,et al.  Handbook of Methods of Applied Statistics , 1968 .

[44]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Phase-entrainment dynamics of visually coupled rhythmic movements , 1994, Biological Cybernetics.

[45]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  Virtual Partner Interaction (VPI): Exploring Novel Behaviors via Coordination Dynamics , 2009, PloS one.

[46]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990 .

[47]  Mario di Bernardo,et al.  Kinematic characteristics of motion in the mirror game , 2014, 2014 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC).

[48]  J. Issartel,et al.  Unintended interpersonal co-ordination: “can we march to the beat of our own drum?” , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.

[49]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .

[50]  Pierre Jacquot,et al.  Phase Extraction in Dynamic Speckle Interferometry with Empirical Mode Decomposition and Hilbert Transform , 2010 .

[51]  P G Zanone,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Coordination: Neural, Behavioral and Social Dynamics , 2008 .

[53]  Robert V. Hogg,et al.  Introduction to Mathematical Statistics. , 1966 .

[54]  Johann Issartel,et al.  A Practical Guide to Time—Frequency Analysis in the Study of Human Motor Behavior: The Contribution of Wavelet Transform , 2006, Journal of motor behavior.

[55]  M. Turvey,et al.  Maintenance tendency in co-ordinated rhythmic movements: Relative fluctuations and phase , 1988, Neuroscience.

[56]  J. Tupin Behavioral physiology. , 1976, Science.

[57]  Gregory W. Corder,et al.  Nonparametric Statistics : A Step-by-Step Approach , 2014 .

[58]  Ludovic Marin,et al.  Social priming enhances interpersonal synchronization and feeling of connectedness towards schizophrenia patients , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[59]  T. Flash,et al.  Moving gracefully: quantitative theories of motor coordination , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[60]  Jason Friedman,et al.  Corrective jitter motion shows similar individual frequencies for the arm and the finger , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[61]  A. Hamilton,et al.  Interference effect of observed human movement on action is due to velocity profile of biological motion , 2007, Social neuroscience.

[62]  Julien Lagarde,et al.  Movement similarities and differences during social interaction: The scientific foundation of the ALTEREGO European project , 2014, 2014 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC).

[63]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Influence of stimulus velocity profile on rhythmic visuomotor coordination. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[64]  D. Brillinger,et al.  Handbook of methods of applied statistics , 1967 .

[65]  Lior Noy,et al.  Being in the zone: physiological markers of togetherness in joint improvisation , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..