Merging different motor patterns: Coordination between rhythmical and discrete single-joint movements

Subjects made fast, discrete elbow flexion movements while simultaneously producing rhythmical oscillations about initial and final visual targets embedded on a horizontal surface. Based on kinematic and electromyographic (EMG) analysis, we found that the discrete movement could start at any phase of the cyclical movement. The most likely onset time occurred when the first agonist burst started at the same moment as a rhythmical burst would have appeared. This resulted in a smooth conjugation between discrete and rhythmical movements. The initiation of the discrete movement was associated with the resetting of the phase of the rhythmical movements. Thus, the time characteristics of the two motor tasks were interdependent. A subset of trials with a uniform distribution of discrete movement onset phases could be selected in most subjects and was averaged to eliminate the cyclical component from the combined movement. Mean kinematic and EMG traces up until the peak velocity were practically identical to those of the discrete movement made alone. The averaging procedure was ineffective in eliminating the rhythmical component following the discrete movement because of the resetting of the phase of oscillation. Using the same procedure it has been shown that initiating the rhythmical movement at the same time as beginning the discrete movement did not affect the initial part of discrete movement. The whole discrete movement was not affected when subjects simultaneously terminated the ongoing rhythmical movements. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that although the rhythmical movement constrains the onset time of discrete movement, the latter, once initiated, proceeds independently of the ongoing rhythmical movement. We also subtracted the discrete component from the combined movement to see how the former affected the rhythmical movement. The residual pattern showed that the rhythmical movements rapidly attenuated when the discrete movement started and then apparently resumed after the peak velocity of the discrete movement. The results corroborate the hypothesis that the control signals underlying the two motor tasks cannot be applied simultaneously, since they may be associated with conflicting stability requirements. Instead, these control signals may be generated sequentially, but the resulting kinematic responses may outlast them and be superposed.

[1]  P. Lafrance,et al.  Digital filters , 1974, Proceedings of the IEEE.

[2]  H. Forssberg Stumbling corrective reaction: a phase-dependent compensatory reaction during locomotion. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  D Goodman,et al.  On the nature of human interlimb coordination. , 1979, Science.

[4]  A. G. Feldman Superposition of motor programs—II. Rapid forearm flexion in man , 1980, Neuroscience.

[5]  A. G. Feldman Superposition of motor programs—I. Rhythmic forearm movements in man , 1980, Neuroscience.

[6]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: effects of practice, uncertainty, and change in target location. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  S. Grillner,et al.  Peripheral control of the cat's step cycle. I. Phase dependent effects of ramp-movements of the hip during "fictive locomotion". , 1981, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[8]  P. Viviani,et al.  Trajectory determines movement dynamics , 1982, Neuroscience.

[9]  Richard B. Stein,et al.  What muscle variable(s) does the nervous system control in limb movements? , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[10]  H. Dr,et al.  Separate cortical systems for control of joint movement and joint stiffness: reciprocal activation and coactivation of antagonist muscles. , 1983 .

[11]  D. Humphrey,et al.  Separate cortical systems for control of joint movement and joint stiffness: reciprocal activation and coactivation of antagonist muscles. , 1983, Advances in neurology.

[12]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Posture control and trajectory formation during arm movement , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  N. Hogan An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Quintern,et al.  Corrective reactions to stumbling in man: Functional significance of spinal and transcortical reflexes , 1984, Neuroscience Letters.

[15]  S Grillner,et al.  Central pattern generators for locomotion, with special reference to vertebrates. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.

[16]  G. A. Robertson,et al.  Blends of rostral and caudal scratch reflex motor patterns elicited by simultaneous stimulation of two sites in the spinal turtle , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  O. I. Fukson,et al.  Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms , 1986, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  S. Rossignol,et al.  A kinematic and electromyographic study of cutaneous reflexes evoked from the forelimb of unrestrained walking cats. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  C. D. De Luca,et al.  Voluntary control of motor units in human antagonist muscles: coactivation and reciprocal activation. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Kinematic strategies and sensorimotor transformations in the wiping movements of frogs. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  J. Flanagan,et al.  The Origin of Electromyograms - Explanations Based on the Equilibrium Point Hypothesis , 1990 .

[23]  Tamar Flash,et al.  The Organization of Human Arm Trajectory Control , 1990 .

[24]  T. Milner,et al.  The effect of accuracy constraints on three-dimensional movement kinematics , 1990, Neuroscience.

[25]  G. L. Gottlieb,et al.  Reconstruction of shifting elbow joint compliant characteristics during fast and slow movements , 1991, Neuroscience.

[26]  P. Crago,et al.  Equilibrium-point hypothesis, minimum effort control strategy and the triphasic muscle activation pattern , 1992 .

[27]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Automatic control of limb movement and posture , 1992, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[28]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Control of Trajectory Modifications in Target-Directed Reaching. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  The patterns of control signals underlying elbow joint movements in humans , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.

[30]  D J Ostry,et al.  Lower pharyngeal wall coarticulation in VCV syllables. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  A. G. Feldman The coactivation command for antagonist muscles involving Ib interneurons in mammalian motor control systems: an electrophysiologically testable model , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.

[32]  M. Latash Control of human movement , 1993 .

[33]  H. Kohase,et al.  Resetting of cortically induced rhythmical jaw movements by stimulation of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus in the guinea pig , 1993, Brain Research.

[34]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  Relativistic effects in single-joint voluntary movements , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.

[35]  Z. Hasan,et al.  Optimized movement trajectories and joint stiffness in unperturbed, inertially loaded movements , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[36]  J. Kelso,et al.  Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  J. Kelso,et al.  Exploring the functional significance of physiological tremor: A biospectroscopic approach , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  H. Nagasaki Asymmetrical trajectory formation in cyclic forearm movements in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Anatol G. Feldman,et al.  Reciprocal and coactivation commands for fast wrist movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  D. Ostry,et al.  Three-dimensional kinematic analysis of frog hindlimb movement in reflex wiping , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.