An Endogenous Circadian Rhythm in Sleep Inertia Results in Greatest Cognitive Impairment upon Awakening during the Biological Night

Sleep inertia is the impaired cognitive performance immediately upon awakening, which decays over tens of minutes. This phenomenon has relevance to people who need to make important decisions soon after awakening, such as on-call emergency workers. Such awakenings can occur at varied times of day or night, so the objective of the study was to determine whether or not the magnitude of sleep inertia varies according to the phase of the endogenous circadian cycle. Twelve adults (mean, 24 years; 7 men) with no medical disorders other than mild asthma were studied. Following 2 baseline days and nights, subjects underwent a forced desynchrony protocol composed of seven 28-h sleep/wake cycles, while maintaining a sleep/wakefulness ratio of 1:2 throughout. Subjects were awakened by a standardized auditory stimulus 3 times each sleep period for sleep inertia assessments. The magnitude of sleep inertia was quantified as the change in cognitive performance (number of correct additions in a 2-min serial addition test) across the first 20 min of wakefulness. Circadian phase was estimated from core body temperature (fitted temperature minimum assigned 0°). Data were segregated according to: (1) circadian phase (60° bins); (2) sleep stage; and (3) 3rd of the night after which awakenings occurred (i.e., tertiary 1, 2, or 3). To control for any effect of sleep stage, the circadian rhythm of sleep inertia was initially assessed following awakenings from Stage 2 (62% of awakening occurred from this stage; n = 110). This revealed a significant circadian rhythm in the sleep inertia of cognitive performance (p = 0.007), which was 3.6 times larger during the biological night (circadian bin 300°, ~2300—0300 h in these subjects) than during the biological day (bin 180°, ~1500—1900 h). The circadian rhythm in sleep inertia was still present when awakenings from all sleep stages were included (p = 0.004), and this rhythm could not be explained by changes in underlying sleep drive prior to awakening (changes in sleep efficiency across circadian phase or across the tertiaries), or by the proportion of the varied sleep stages prior to awakenings. This robust endogenous circadian rhythm in sleep inertia may have important implications for people who need to be alert soon after awakening.

[1]  C A Czeisler,et al.  Short‐term memory, alertness and performance: a reappraisal of their relationship to body temperature , 1992, Journal of sleep research.

[2]  D. Dinges,et al.  Caffeine eliminates psychomotor vigilance deficits from sleep inertia. , 2001, Sleep.

[3]  A. Rechtschaffen,et al.  A manual of standardized terminology, technique and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects , 1968 .

[4]  Charles A Czeisler,et al.  Effects of sleep inertia on cognition. , 2006, JAMA.

[5]  A. Rechtschaffen A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring system for sleep of human subjects , 1968 .

[6]  Derk-Jan Dijk,et al.  Paradoxical timing of the circadian rhythm of sleep propensity serves to consolidate sleep and wakefulness in humans , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  Richard E Kronauer,et al.  Comparisons of the Variability of Three Markers of the Human Circadian Pacemaker , 2002, Journal of biological rhythms.

[8]  Alain Muzet,et al.  Sleep inertia. , 2000, Sleep medicine reviews.

[9]  A. Muzet,et al.  Sleep inertia. , 2000, Sleep medicine reviews.

[10]  Alain Muzet,et al.  EEG spectral power and cognitive performance during sleep inertia: The effect of normal sleep duration and partial sleep deprivation , 2006, Physiology & Behavior.

[11]  W. B. Webb,et al.  Reaction Time and Serial Response Efficiency on Arousal from Sleep , 1964, Perceptual and motor skills.

[12]  Yan Zhu,et al.  A neural circuit for circadian regulation of arousal , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  A A Borbely,et al.  Time course of sleep inertia after nighttime and daytime sleep episodes. , 1995, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[14]  Kazuhiko Fukuda,et al.  Proposed supplements and amendments to ‘A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects’, the Rechtschaffen & Kales (1968) standard , 2001, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[15]  Christopher Drake,et al.  Ethanol and sleep loss: a "dose" comparison of impairing effects. , 2003, Sleep.

[16]  H Eugene Stanley,et al.  Non-random fluctuations and multi-scale dynamics regulation of human activity. , 2004, Physica A.

[17]  David F. Dinges,et al.  Assessing performance upon abrupt awakening from naps during quasi-continuous operations , 1985 .

[18]  H. Babkoff,et al.  Sleep inertia: best time not to wake up? , 1993, Chronobiology international.

[19]  J. Tanji,et al.  Behavioral planning in the prefrontal cortex , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[20]  M. Carskadon,et al.  Neurobehavioral performance of residents after heavy night call vs after alcohol ingestion. , 2005, JAMA.

[21]  H Eugene Stanley,et al.  Endogenous circadian rhythm in an index of cardiac vulnerability independent of changes in behavior , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  D. Bruck,et al.  The effects of sleep inertia on decision‐making performance , 1999, Journal of sleep research.

[23]  C A Czeisler,et al.  Time course of sleep inertia dissipation in human performance and alertness , 1999, Journal of sleep research.

[24]  R. Kronauer,et al.  Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker. , 1999, Science.

[25]  Fawaz Ahmad,et al.  The Effects of On-Duty Napping on Intern Sleep Time and Fatigue , 2006, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[26]  Peter Herscovitch,et al.  The process of awakening: a PET study of regional brain activity patterns mediating the re-establishment of alertness and consciousness. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.