High Level of Construal and Psychological Distance Reduce Melioration

We examined how level of construal and psychological distance affected performance in a task in which value of alternatives depended on frequency of choice. Melioration, a sub-optimal choice strategy that fails to take into account such value changes, has been found to be prevalent and difficult to change. Participants repeatedly chose between two buttons to earn as many coins as possible. Button A earned N coins, button B earned N + 3 coins, and N was the number of A presses in the last five trials. In this situation, B has a higher immediate payoff, but because choosing A increases the return from both buttons for the next five trials, maximizing A choices is the optimal strategy. We predicted that because a global perspective is needed to appreciate the dependency of value on frequency of choice, frequency of choosing the globally maximizing alternative (A) would increase when participants apply a higher level of construal and assume a more psychologically distal perspective. Study 1 manipulated construal level through segmenting the sequence of trials into segments of different size, with bigger segments representing higher level of construal. In Study 2, participants played a practice game, and we manipulated the probability of playing the actual game. Both a higher level of construal and a lower probability of playing the actual game (higher distance) increased the number of A choices. These results suggest that a broad perspective that is induced by high level of construal or by psychological distancing may reduce sub-optimal, meliorating choice

[1]  K. Fujita,et al.  Transcending Temptation Through Abstraction , 2012 .

[2]  Y. Trope,et al.  Levels of mental construal , 2012 .

[3]  G. Kalyanaram,et al.  Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness , 2011 .

[4]  Kentaro Fujita,et al.  Promoting prospective self-control through abstraction ☆ , 2010 .

[5]  K. Fujita,et al.  Seeing the Big Picture: A Construal Level Analysis of Self-Control , 2010 .

[6]  Y. Trope,et al.  Construal-level theory of psychological distance. , 2010, Psychological review.

[7]  K. Fujita,et al.  Construals and Prospective Self-Control 1 RUNNING HEAD : CONSTRUALS AND PROSPECTIVE SELF-CONTROL Promoting Prospective Self-Control Through Abstraction , 2010 .

[8]  Richard J Tunney,et al.  Melioration behaviour in the Harvard game is reduced by simplifying decision outcomes , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  Y. Trope,et al.  The Psychology of Transcending the Here and Now , 2008, Science.

[10]  Ying Zhang,et al.  Together or apart: when goals and temptations complement versus compete. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  Y. Trope,et al.  Seeing the forest when entry is unlikely: probability and the mental representation of events. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[12]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Construal levels and self-control. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Spatial Distance and Mental Construal of Social Events , 2006, Psychological science.

[14]  J. Polivy,et al.  The self-regulation of eating: Theoretical and practical problems. , 2004 .

[15]  Daniel Gopher,et al.  Melioration and the Transition from Touch-Typing Training to Everyday Use , 2003, Hum. Factors.

[16]  Y. Trope,et al.  The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. , 2002 .

[17]  D. Shanks,et al.  A re‐examination of melioration and rational choice , 2002 .

[18]  Howard Rachlin,et al.  Rule-governed versus contingency-governed behavior in a self-control task: effects of changes in contingencies , 2002, Behavioural Processes.

[19]  Karen Gasper,et al.  Attending to the Big Picture: Mood and Global Versus Local Processing of Visual Information , 2002, Psychological science.

[20]  Nira Liberman,et al.  Abstract and concrete self-evaluative goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  Bob Remington,et al.  When more means less: factors affecting human self-control in a local versus global choice paradigm , 1999 .

[22]  Jeremy R. Gray,et al.  A Bias Toward Short-Term Thinking in Threat-Related Negative Emotional States , 1999 .

[23]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. , 1999, Psychological review.

[24]  Y. Trope,et al.  The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. , 1998 .

[25]  Howard Rachlin,et al.  Temporal Patterning in Choice among Delayed Outcomes , 1996 .

[26]  H. Rachlin The Value of Temporal Patterns in Behavior , 1995 .

[27]  Howard Rachlin,et al.  Self-control: Beyond commitment. , 1995 .

[28]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Diversification bias: Explaining the discrepancy in variety seeking between combined and separated choices , 1995 .

[29]  R. Herrnstein,et al.  Utility maximization and melioration: Internalities in individual choice , 1993 .

[30]  I. Simonson The Effect of Purchase Quantity and Timing on Variety-Seeking Behavior , 1990 .

[31]  M. Doherty,et al.  Effects of cognitive feedback on performance. , 1989 .

[32]  W. Mischel,et al.  Delay of gratification in children. , 1989, Science.

[33]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.