German inherent datives and argument structure

Two lines of recent research are brought together to argue that the defining characteristic of German inherent dative arguments is not morphological but syntactic. On the one hand, evidence is mounting that dative objects with two-place verbs, as well as those with three-place verbs, differ in their argument-structural status from direct objects receiving structural accusative case. On the other hand, the GB idea that movement is case-driven, which was central to previous analyses of the behavior of inherent datives under passivization, has been considerably weakened. It is proposed instead that the special syntactic, semantic and morphological properties of dative arguments should be derived from the way in which they are introduced into the structure. This not only accounts for the German data that form the basis of the discussion, but can also be more easily extended to languages like Icelandic, which are famously problematic for the original inherent Case analysis.

[1]  R. Larson On the double object construction , 1988 .

[2]  Thórhallur Eythórsson,et al.  The change that never happened: the story of oblique subjects , 2003, Journal of Linguistics.

[3]  Morris Halle,et al.  Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection , 1993 .

[4]  Noam Chomsky Derivation by phase , 1999 .

[5]  J. Maling Dative: The heterogeneity of the mapping among morphological case, grammatical functions, and thematic roles , 2001 .

[6]  Johanna Barddal,et al.  “Oblique Subjects” in Icelandic and German , 2002 .

[7]  Alec Marantz,et al.  Case and licensing , 2000 .

[8]  Hubert Haider,et al.  Deutsche Syntax : generativ : Vorstudien zur Theorie einer projektiven Grammatik , 1993 .

[9]  A. Marantz Implications of asymmetries in double object constructions , 1993 .

[10]  Halldor Armann,et al.  Icelandic non-nominative subjects Facts and implications , 2007 .

[11]  Alice C. Andrews The VP-complement analysis in modern Icelandic , 1990 .

[12]  Maria Cristina Cuervo,et al.  Datives at large , 2003 .

[13]  Peter Svenonius,et al.  Icelandic Case and the Structure of Events , 2002 .

[14]  Gisbert Fanselow,et al.  Quirky “subjects” and other specifiers , 2002 .

[15]  Thomas McFadden,et al.  The position of morphological *case in the derivation: A study on the syntax -morphology interface , 2004 .

[16]  R. Jackendoff,et al.  Control Is Not Movement , 2001, Linguistic Inquiry.

[17]  Kerstin Blume A contrastive analysis of interaction verbs with dative complements , 1998 .

[18]  H. Sigurðsson Verbal Syntax and Case in Icelandic , 1989 .

[19]  Elena Anagnostopoulou,et al.  Progress in Grammar. Articles at the 20th Anniversary of the Comparison of Grammatical Models Group in Tilburg , 2001 .

[20]  Elena Anagnostopoulou,et al.  TWO CLASSES OF DOUBLE OBJECT VERBS : THE ROLE OF ZERO MORPHOLOGY , 2001 .

[21]  I. Landau Movement Out of Control , 2003, Linguistic Inquiry.

[22]  Mark C. Baker,et al.  Thematic Roles and Syntactic Structure , 1997 .

[23]  Anna Roussou,et al.  A minimalist theory of A-movement and control , 2000 .

[24]  Johanna Barddal,et al.  Case in Icelandic : A Synchronic, Diachronic and Comparative Approach , 2001 .

[25]  Stefan Müller,et al.  An HPSG Analysis of German Depictive Secondary Predicates , 2004, FGMOL.

[26]  Dieter Wunderlich dem Freund die Hand auf die Schulter legen , 1996 .

[27]  Susanne Wurmbrand,et al.  Infinitives: Restructuring and Clause Structure , 2001 .

[28]  N. Hornstein Movement and Control , 1999, Linguistic Inquiry.

[29]  I. Landau Elements of control , 2000 .

[30]  Carson T. Schütze INFL in child and adult language : agreement, case and licensing , 1997 .

[31]  A. Barss,et al.  A note on anaphora and double objects , 1986 .

[32]  C. Reiss,et al.  The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Interfaces , 2007 .

[33]  Hubert Haider,et al.  OV is More Basic than VO , 2000 .

[34]  Arthur Stepanov,et al.  On the quirky difference Icelandic vs. German: A note of doubt , 2003 .

[35]  Andrew McIntyre,et al.  The interpretation of German datives and English have , 2006 .

[36]  Daniel Hole Reconciling “possessor” datives and “beneficiary” datives – Towards a unified voice account of dative binding in German , 2006 .

[37]  Stanley Dubinsky,et al.  Abstracting Away from Abstract Case , 2000 .

[38]  Josef Bayer,et al.  Morphological underspecification meets oblique case : syntactic and processing effects in German , 2001 .

[39]  Jürgen Lenerz,et al.  Zur Abfolge nominaler Satzglieder im Deutschen , 1977 .

[40]  André Meinunger Syntactic aspects of topic and comment , 2000 .

[41]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  Predication , 2001, Cogn. Sci..

[42]  W. Abraham,et al.  Deutsche Syntax im Sprachenvergleich : Grundlegung einer typologischen Syntax des Deutschen , 1995 .

[43]  André Meinunger Remarks on the projection of dative arguments in German , 2006 .

[44]  Halldor Armann Sigurðsson,et al.  Case: abstract vs. morphological , 2003 .

[45]  A. Zaenen,et al.  Case and grammatical functions: The Icelandic passive , 1985 .

[46]  þrír málvísindamenn,et al.  Agree and Agreement : Evidence from Germanic , 2005 .

[47]  Adriana Belletti,et al.  The Case of Unaccusatives , 1988 .

[48]  László Molnarfi,et al.  Kasusstrukturalität und struktureller kasus- zur lage des dativs im heutigen deutsch: 1717 , 1998 .

[49]  Roger Martin,et al.  A minimalist theory of PRO and control , 1996 .

[50]  Hubert Haider The License to License: Licensing of structural case plus economy yields Burzio’s Generalization , 2000 .

[51]  Jindrich Toman,et al.  The Case of German , 1985 .

[52]  H. Harley Subjects, events, and licensing , 1995 .

[53]  Moira Yip,et al.  Case in Tiers , 1987 .

[54]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The Minimalist Program , 1992 .

[55]  H. Haider,et al.  Precedence among predicates , 1997 .

[56]  Roger Martin,et al.  Null Case and the Distribution of PRO , 2001, Linguistic Inquiry.

[57]  Cedric Boeckx,et al.  Reply to Control Is Not Movement , 2003, Linguistic Inquiry.

[58]  H. Sigurðsson The case of quirky subjects , 1992 .