Using real-world scenes as contextual cues for search

Research on contextual cueing has demonstrated that with simple arrays of letters and shapes, search for a target increases in efficiency as associations between a search target and its surrounding visual context are learned. We investigated whether the visual context afforded by repeated exposure to real-world scenes can also guide attention when the relationship between the scene and a target position is arbitrary. Observers searched for and identified a target letter embedded in photographs of real-world scenes. Although search time within novel scenes was consistent across trials, search time within repeated scenes decreased across repetitions. Unlike previous demonstrations of contextual cueing, however, memory for scene-target covariation was explicit. In subsequent memory tests, observers recognized repeated contexts more often than those that were presented once and displayed superior recall of target position within the repeated scenes. In addition, repetition of inverted scenes, which made the scene more difficult to identify, produced a markedly reduced rate of learning, suggesting semantic information concerning object and scene identity are used to guide attention.

[1]  R. Shepard Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures , 1967 .

[2]  L. Standing Learning 10000 pictures , 1973 .

[3]  L. Standing Learning 10,000 pictures. , 1973, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  I. Rock The perception of disoriented figures. , 1974, Scientific American.

[5]  D. Hamlyn,et al.  Cognitive systems, ‘folk psychology’, and knowledge , 1981, Cognition.

[6]  R. Klein Patterns of perceived similarity cannot be generalized from long to short exposure durations and vice versa , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  H. Intraub Conceptual masking: the effects of subsequent visual events on memory for pictures. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  Z W Pylyshyn,et al.  Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.

[9]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Integration and accumulation of information across saccadic eye movements. , 1996 .

[10]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.

[11]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  J. Henderson,et al.  High-level scene perception. , 1999, Annual review of psychology.

[13]  M. Chun,et al.  Top-Down Attentional Guidance Based on Implicit Learning of Visual Covariation , 1999 .

[14]  R. Klein,et al.  The Effects of Scene Inversion on Change Blindness , 2000, The Journal of general psychology.

[15]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual cueing of visual attention , 2022 .

[16]  M. Chun,et al.  The spatial gradient of visual masking by object substitution , 2001, Vision Research.

[17]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Visual Search has Memory , 2001, Psychological science.

[18]  M. Chun,et al.  Perceptual constraints on implicit learning of spatial context , 2002 .

[19]  M. Chun,et al.  Implicit, long-term spatial contextual memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  M. Chun Scene Perception and Memory , 2003 .

[21]  Marvin M Chun,et al.  Implicit scene learning is viewpoint dependent , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  How Much Memory Does Oculomotor Search Have? , 2003, Psychological science.

[23]  D. Simons,et al.  What Makes Change Blindness Interesting , 2003 .