Localizing nearby sound sources in a classroom: binaural room impulse responses.

Binaural room impulse responses (BRIRs) were measured in a classroom for sources at different azimuths and distances (up to 1 m) relative to a manikin located in four positions in a classroom. When the listener is far from all walls, reverberant energy distorts signal magnitude and phase independently at each frequency, altering monaural spectral cues, interaural phase differences, and interaural level differences. For the tested conditions, systematic distortion (comb-filtering) from an early intense reflection is only evident when a listener is very close to a wall, and then only in the ear facing the wall. Especially for a nearby source, interaural cues grow less reliable with increasing source laterality and monaural spectral cues are less reliable in the ear farther from the sound source. Reverberation reduces the magnitude of interaural level differences at all frequencies; however, the direct-sound interaural time difference can still be recovered from the BRIRs measured in these experiments. Results suggest that bias and variability in sound localization behavior may vary systematically with listener location in a room as well as source location relative to the listener, even for nearby sources where there is relatively little reverberant energy.

[1]  Toshiyuki Okano,et al.  Judgments of noticeable differences in sound fields of concert halls caused by intensity variations in early reflections. , 2002, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  Mendel Kleiner,et al.  Auralization-An Overview , 1993 .

[3]  John Vanderkooy,et al.  Aspects of MLS Measuring Systems , 1994 .

[4]  Chaz Yee Toh,et al.  Effects of reverberation on perceptual segregation of competing voices. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  S. Bech,et al.  Timbral aspects of reproduced sound in small rooms. I. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  F. Asano,et al.  Role of spectral cues in median plane localization. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  F L Wightman,et al.  Headphone simulation of free-field listening. I: Stimulus synthesis. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  Tara J. Brown Characterization of acoustic head-related transfer functions for nearby sources , 2000 .

[9]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Auditory Localization in Rooms: Acoustic Analysis and Behavior , 2002 .

[10]  F. Wightman,et al.  A model of head-related transfer functions based on principal components analysis and minimum-phase reconstruction. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  Nathaniel I. Durlach,et al.  Chapter 11 – MODELS OF BINAURAL INTERACTION , 1978 .

[12]  J. S. Bradley,et al.  On the importance of early reflections for speech in rooms. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  Richard L Freyman,et al.  Auditory target detection in reverberation. , 2004, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  R. Humanski,et al.  Localization of sound in the vertical plane with and without high-frequency spectral cues , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham SPEECH INTELLIGIBILITY, SPATIAL UNMASKING, AND REALISM IN REVERBERANT SPATIAL AUDITORY DISPLAYS , 2002 .

[16]  M. Kleiner,et al.  Computation of edge diffraction for more accurate room acoustics auralization. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  M. Schroeder New Method of Measuring Reverberation Time , 1965 .

[18]  S. Bech,et al.  Spatial aspects of reproduced sound in small rooms. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Identifying where you are in a room: Sensitivity to room acoustics , 2003 .

[20]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  The Perceptual Consequences of Creating a Realistic, Reverberant 3-D Audio Display , 2003 .

[21]  Marc Naguib,et al.  Auditory distance assessment of singing conspecifies in Carolina wrens: the role of reverberation and frequency-dependent attenuation , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  Michael Barron,et al.  Late lateral energy fractions and the envelopment question in concert halls , 2001 .

[23]  Gerald A. Studebaker,et al.  Acoustical Factors Affecting Hearing Aid Performance , 1992 .

[24]  C Trahiotis,et al.  Lateralization of bands of noise: effects of bandwidth and differences of interaural time and phase. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  John S. Bradley,et al.  The influence of late arriving energy on spatial impression , 1995 .

[26]  Beranek,et al.  Objective and subjective evaluations of twenty-three opera houses in Europe, Japan, and the Americas , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  M. Morimoto,et al.  The contribution of two ears to the perception of vertical angle in sagittal planes. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  Brad Rakerd,et al.  Localization of sound in reverberant spaces , 1999 .

[29]  John Vanderkooy,et al.  Transfer-Function Measurement with Maximum-Length Sequences , 1989 .

[30]  Mark R. Anderson,et al.  Direct comparison of the impact of head tracking, reverberation, and individualized head-related transfer functions on the spatial perception of a virtual speech source. , 2001, Journal of the Audio Engineering Society. Audio Engineering Society.

[31]  B. Shinn-Cunningham DISTANCE CUES FOR VIRTUAL AUDITORY SPACE , 2000 .

[32]  W. Hartmann,et al.  Binaural coherence in rooms , 2005 .

[33]  Barbara G Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Spatial unmasking of nearby pure-tone targets in a simulated anechoic environment. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[34]  T N Buell,et al.  Combination of binaural information across frequency bands. , 1991, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[35]  J. S. Bradley,et al.  Speech intelligibility studies in classrooms. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[36]  Pavel Zahorik,et al.  Assessing auditory distance perception using virtual acoustics. , 2002, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[37]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  PERCEPTUAL CONSENQUECES OF INCLUDING REVERBERATION IN SPATIAL AUDITORY DISPLAYS , 2003 .

[38]  J. S. Bradley Some effects of orchestra shells , 1996 .

[39]  Murray Hodgson,et al.  Experimental investigation of the acoustical characteristics of university classrooms , 1999 .

[40]  Tammo Houtgast,et al.  Auditory distance perception in rooms , 1999, Nature.

[41]  Masanao Ebata,et al.  Spatial unmasking and attention related to the cocktail party problem , 2003 .

[42]  Nathaniel I. Durlach,et al.  On the Externalization of Auditory Images , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[43]  R. Duda,et al.  Range dependence of the response of a spherical head model , 1998 .

[44]  J S Bradley,et al.  On the combined effects of signal-to-noise ratio and room acoustics on speech intelligibility. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[45]  B. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Neural representation of source direction in reverberant space , 2003, 2003 IEEE Workshop on Applications of Signal Processing to Audio and Acoustics (IEEE Cat. No.03TH8684).

[46]  A. D. Little,et al.  Effects of Room Reflectance and Background Noise on Perceived Auditory Distance , 1989, Perception.

[47]  Abhijit Kulkarni,et al.  Infinite-impulse-response models of the head-related transfer function. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[48]  A. Nabelek,et al.  Reverberant overlap- and self-masking in consonant identification. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[49]  J. C. Middlebrooks,et al.  Listener weighting of cues for lateral angle: the duplex theory of sound localization revisited. , 2002, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[50]  B. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Tori of confusion: binaural localization cues for sources within reach of a listener. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[51]  J. C. Middlebrooks Virtual localization improved by scaling nonindividualized external-ear transfer functions in frequency. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[52]  A John Van Opstal,et al.  The influence of duration and level on human sound localization. , 2004, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[53]  L L Beranek,et al.  Mechanism of sound absorption by seated audience in halls. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[54]  F L Wightman,et al.  Monaural sound localization revisited. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[55]  B G Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Spatial unmasking of nearby speech sources in a simulated anechoic environment. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[56]  D. Brungart Auditory localization of nearby sources. III. Stimulus effects. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[57]  Manfred R. Schroeder,et al.  Statistical parameters of the frequency response curves of large rooms , 1987 .

[58]  W. Hartmann,et al.  The role of reverberation in release from masking due to spatial separation of sources for speech identification , 2005 .

[59]  J. S. Bradley,et al.  Reverberation time and maximum background-noise level for classrooms from a comparative study of speech intelligibility metrics. , 2000, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[60]  W. Hartmann Localization of sound in rooms. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[61]  D. M. Green,et al.  Sound localization by human listeners. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[62]  M. Schroeder,et al.  On Frequency Response Curves in Rooms. Comparison of Experimental, Theoretical, and Monte Carlo Results for the Average Frequency Spacing between Maxima , 1962 .

[63]  W. M. Rabinowitz,et al.  Auditory localization of nearby sources. Head-related transfer functions. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[64]  John F. Culling,et al.  Effects of simulated reverberation on the use of binaural cues and fundamental-frequency differences for separating concurrent vowels , 1994, Speech Commun..

[65]  Pavel Zahorik,et al.  Direct-to-reverberant energy ratio sensitivity. , 2002, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[66]  Jont B. Allen,et al.  Image method for efficiently simulating small‐room acoustics , 1976 .

[67]  R W Hukin,et al.  Effects of reverberation on spatial, prosodic, and vocal-tract size cues to selective attention. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[68]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Spatial unmasking of nearby speech sources in a simulated anechoic environment , 2000 .

[69]  W. M. Rabinowitz,et al.  Auditory localization of nearby sources. II. Localization of a broadband source. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[70]  Jan Baan,et al.  Spatial fluctuations in measures for spaciousness , 2001 .

[71]  R. Butler,et al.  Spectral cues utilized in the localization of sound in the median sagittal plane. , 1977, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.