Characterizing individual differences in heat-pain sensitivity

Abstract Heat induced pain has been shown to follow a positively accelerating power function for groups of subjects, yet the extent to which this applies to individual subjects is unknown. Statistical methods were developed for assessing the goodness of fit and reliability of the power function for data from individual subjects with the aim of using such functions for characterizing individual differences in heat‐pain sensitivity. 175 subjects rated ascending and random series of contact heat stimuli with visual analogue scales for pain intensity (VAS‐I) and unpleasantness (VAS‐A). Curve fitting showed excellent model fit. Substitution of model estimates in place of observed VAS scores produced minimal bias in group means, about 0.3 VAS units in the ascending series and 1.0 in the random series, on a 0–100 scale. Individual power function exponents were considerably higher for the ascending than for the random series and somewhat higher for VAS‐A than for VAS‐I (means: ascending VAS‐I=9.04, VAS‐A=9.80; random VAS‐I=4.95, VAS‐A=5.67). The reliability of VAS estimates was high (≧.93), and for the ascending series it remained so when extrapolating 4 °C beyond the empirical range. Exponent reliability was high for the ascending series (VAS‐I=.92; VAS‐A=.91), but considerably lower for the random series (VAS‐I=.69; VAS‐A=.71). Individual differences constituted 60% of the total variance in pain ratings, whereas stimulus temperature accounted for only 40%. This finding underscores the importance of taking individual differences into account when performing pain studies.

[1]  David A. Williams,et al.  Evidence of augmented central pain processing in idiopathic chronic low back pain. , 2004, Arthritis and rheumatism.

[2]  Donald D. Price,et al.  A comparison of pain measurement characteristics of mechanical visual analogue and simple numerical rating scales , 1994, Pain.

[3]  C. Vierck,et al.  Ratings of experimental pain and pain-related negative affect predict clinical pain in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome , 2003, Pain.

[4]  P. Leino-Arjas,et al.  Possible association of interleukin 1 gene locus polymorphisms with low back pain , 2004, Pain.

[5]  Daniel J Clauw,et al.  Increased pain sensitivity in fibromyalgia: effects of stimulus type and mode of presentation , 2003, Pain.

[6]  P. Magnus,et al.  The Norwegian Institute of Public Health Twin Panel: a description of the sample and program of research. , 2002, Twin research : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies.

[7]  W. Maixner,et al.  Genetic basis for individual variations in pain perception and the development of a chronic pain condition. , 2005, Human molecular genetics.

[8]  G. Giesler,et al.  Spinothalamic and spinohypothalamic tract neurons in the cervical enlargement of rats. II. Responses to innocuous and noxious mechanical and thermal stimuli. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  G. Carli,et al.  Reactivity to superficial and deep stimuli in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain , 2002, Pain.

[10]  W. Clark,et al.  On the absence of correlation between responses to noxious heat, cold, electrical and ischemie stimulation , 1994, Pain.

[11]  Spatial summation of heat pain: a reassessment , 1996, Pain.

[12]  P. Magnus,et al.  The Norwegian Institute of Public Health Twin Panel: A Description of the Sample and Program of Research , 2002, Twin Research.

[13]  R. Fillingim,et al.  Sex differences in heat pain thresholds as a function of assessment method and rate of rise. , 1999, Somatosensory & motor research.

[14]  J. Olesen,et al.  Qualitatively altered nociception in chronic myofascial pain , 1996, Pain.

[15]  Joshua A. Bueller,et al.  COMT val158met Genotype Affects µ-Opioid Neurotransmitter Responses to a Pain Stressor , 2003, Science.

[16]  David Goldman,et al.  Genetic influence on variability in human acute experimental pain sensitivity associated with gender, ethnicity and psychological temperament , 2004, Pain.

[17]  D. Price,et al.  The validation of visual analogue scales as ratio scale measures for chronic and experimental pain , 1983, Pain.

[18]  S S Stevens,et al.  To Honor Fechner and Repeal His Law: A power function, not a log function, describes the operating characteristic of a sensory system. , 1961, Science.

[19]  Robert C. Coghill,et al.  Neural correlates of interindividual differences in the subjective experience of pain , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  R. Coghill,et al.  Wide dynamic range but not nociceptive-specific neurons encode multidimensional features of prolonged repetitive heat pain. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Robert Tibshirani,et al.  Bootstrap Methods for Standard Errors, Confidence Intervals, and Other Measures of Statistical Accuracy , 1986 .