Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns

We report a functional neuroimaging study with positron emission tomography (PET) in which six healthy adult volunteers were scanned while watching silent computer-presented animations. The characters in the animations were simple geometrical shapes whose movement patterns selectively evoked mental state attribution or simple action description. Results showed increased activation in association with mental state attribution in four main regions: medial prefrontal cortex, temporoparietal junction (superior temporal sulcus), basal temporal regions (fusiform gyrus and temporal poles adjacent to the amygdala), and extrastriate cortex (occipital gyrus). Previous imaging studies have implicated these regions in self-monitoring, in the perception of biological motion, and in the attribution of mental states using verbal stimuli or visual depictions of the human form. We suggest that these regions form a network for processing information about intentions, and speculate that the ability to make inferences about other people's mental states evolved from the ability to make inferences about other creatures' actions.

[1]  F. Heider,et al.  An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .

[2]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[3]  M. Mintun,et al.  Noninvasive functional brain mapping by change-distribution analysis of averaged PET images of H215O tissue activity. , 1989, Journal of nuclear medicine : official publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine.

[4]  D. Sperber,et al.  Relevance: Communication and Cognition , 1989 .

[5]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Relationship between Global and Local Changes in PET Scans , 1990, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[6]  E. Hoffman,et al.  Fully three-dimensional reconstruction for a PET camera with retractable septa , 1991 .

[7]  J. Morton,et al.  The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: autism , 1991, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  Alan M. Leslie,et al.  Domain specificity in conceptual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism , 1992, Cognition.

[9]  Stephen J. Misovich,et al.  Effects of Disruption of Structure and Motion on Perceptions of Social Causality , 1992 .

[10]  John P. Aggleton,et al.  The amygdala: Neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction. , 1992 .

[11]  J. Fodor A theory of the child's theory of mind , 1992, Cognition.

[12]  A. Syrota,et al.  The Cortical Representation of Speech , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  F. Happé Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory , 1993, Cognition.

[14]  Ken Springer,et al.  Structure, Motion, and Preschoolers' Perceptions of Social Causality , 1993 .

[15]  T. Allison,et al.  Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe , 1994, Nature.

[16]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[17]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Understanding other minds : perspectives from autism , 1994 .

[18]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Recognition of Mental State Terms , 1994, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[19]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of figurative aspects of language. A positron emission tomography activation study. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[21]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[22]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[23]  Peter K. Smith,et al.  Theories of Theories of Mind , 1996 .

[24]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures , 1996, Nature.

[25]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Specific Involvement of Human Parietal Systems and the Amygdala in the Perception of Biological Motion , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  ‘Theory of mind’ in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome , 1996, Neuroreport.

[27]  Philip McGuire,et al.  Brain activity during stimulus independent thought. , 1996 .

[28]  T. Allison,et al.  Differential Sensitivity of Human Visual Cortex to Faces, Letterstrings, and Textures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  David Silbersweig,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of verbal self-monitoring , 1996 .

[30]  U. Frith,et al.  The neuropsychology of autism. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Activation of auditory cortex during silent lipreading. , 1997, Science.

[32]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses , 1997, Neuroreport.

[33]  C D Frith,et al.  Neural mechanisms involved in the processing of global and local aspects of hierarchically organized visual stimuli. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Bushnell,et al.  Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. , 1997, Science.

[36]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Segregating Semantic from Phonological Processes during Reading , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  J. Grafman,et al.  A study of the performance of patients with frontal lobe lesions in a financial planning task. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[39]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[40]  C J Price,et al.  The neural systems sustaining face and proper-name processing. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  T. Allison,et al.  Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  J. Decety,et al.  Top down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a pet investigation. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[43]  T. Paus,et al.  Regional differences in the effects of task difficulty and motor output on blood flow response in the human anterior cingulate cortex: a review of 107 PET activation studies , 1998, Neuroreport.

[44]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A multimodal language region in the ventral visual pathway , 1998, Nature.

[46]  Cheryl L. Grady,et al.  Neuroimaging and activation of the frontal lobes. , 1999 .

[47]  H. Duvernoy,et al.  The Human Brain: Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy with MRI, and Blood Supply , 1999 .

[48]  Brian J. Scholl,et al.  Modularity, Development and ‘Theory of Mind’ , 1999 .

[49]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[50]  U. Frith,et al.  Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development , 2000 .

[51]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience, 2nd ed. , 2000 .

[52]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.