Toddlers Always Get the Last Word: Recency biases in early verbal behavior

A popular conception about language development is that comprehension precedes production. Although this is certainly true during the earliest stages of phonological development, once a child possesses the basic articulatory skills required for imitation, it need not necessarily be the case. A child could produce a word without possessing the fully formed lexical representation through imitation. In some cases, such as in response to questions containing fixed choices, this behavior could be mistaken for a deeper understanding of the words’ semantic content. In this paper, we present evidence that 2to 3-year-old children exhibit a robust recency bias when verbally responding to two-alternative choice questions (i.e., they select the second, most recently mentioned option), possibly due to the availability of the second word in phonological memory. We find further evidence of this effect outside of a laboratory setting in naturalistic conversational contexts in CHILDES (MacWhinney, 2000), a large corpus of transcribed child-adult interactions.

[1]  Alicia J. Knoedler,et al.  Distinctiveness and Serial Position Effects in Recognition and Sentence Processing , 1994 .

[2]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The phonological loop as a language learning device. , 1998, Psychological review.

[3]  R. Brown,et al.  A First Language , 1973 .

[4]  L. Camras,et al.  Production of emotional facial expressions in European American, Japanese, and Chinese infants. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[5]  F. Keil Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development , 1989 .

[6]  Björn-Olav Dozo,et al.  Quantitative Analysis of Culture Using Millions of Digitized Books , 2010 .

[7]  Murdock,et al.  The serial position effect of free recall , 1962 .

[8]  Jennifer Culbertson,et al.  Word-minimality, Epenthesis and Coda Licensing in the Early Acquisition of English , 2006, Language and speech.

[9]  Robert L. Greene,et al.  Sources of recency effects in free recall. , 1986 .

[10]  D. Jackson-Maldonado,et al.  MacArthur‐Bates Communicative Development Inventories , 2012 .

[11]  Virginia A. Marchman,et al.  MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories , 2006 .

[12]  Harriet Oster,et al.  Japanese and American infants' responses to arm restraint. , 1992 .

[13]  D. Swingley,et al.  At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  J. Michael Verbal behavior. , 1984, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[15]  Alan Cruttenden,et al.  Falls and rises: meanings and universals , 1981, Journal of Linguistics.

[16]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[17]  J. Deese,et al.  Serial effects in recall of unorganized and sequentially organized verbal material. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  Colin Bannard,et al.  Stored Word Sequences in Language Learning , 2008, Psychological science.

[19]  Bob McMurray,et al.  Defusing the Childhood Vocabulary Explosion , 2007, Science.

[20]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk (third edition): Volume I: Transcription format and programs, Volume II: The database , 2000, Computational Linguistics.