Selective Attention to Affective Value Alters How the Brain Processes Olfactory Stimuli

How does selective attention to affect influence sensory processing? In a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation, when subjects were instructed to remember and rate the pleasantness of a jasmin odor, activations were greater in the medial orbito-frontal and pregenual cingulate cortex than when subjects were instructed to remember and rate the intensity of the odor. When the subjects were instructed to remember and rate the intensity, activations were greater in the inferior frontal gyrus. These top-down effects occurred not only during odor delivery but started in a preparation period after the instruction before odor delivery, and continued after termination of the odor in a short-term memory period. Thus, depending on the context in which odors are presented and whether affect is relevant, the brain prepares itself, responds to, and remembers an odor differently. These findings show that when attention is paid to affective value, the brain systems engaged to prepare for, represent, and remember a sensory stimulus are different from those engaged when attention is directed to the physical properties of a stimulus such as its intensity. This differential biasing of brain regions engaged in processing a sensory stimulus depending on whether the cognitive demand is for affect-related versus more sensory-related processing may be an important aspect of cognition and attention. This has many implications for understanding the effects not only of olfactory but also of other sensory stimuli.

[1]  Hilda M. Fehd,et al.  Mechanisms of feature- and space-based attention: response modulation and baseline increases. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[3]  E. Rolls Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making: A unifying computational neuroscience approach , 2007 .

[4]  J. Price,et al.  The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  E. Rolls,et al.  Cognitive Modulation of Olfactory Processing , 2005, Neuron.

[6]  Ralf Deichmann,et al.  Functional Heterogeneity in Human Olfactory Cortex: An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  A. Holley,et al.  Functional Neuroanatomy of Different Olfactory Judgments , 2001, NeuroImage.

[8]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Dissociable Codes of Odor Quality and Odorant Structure in Human Piriform Cortex , 2006, Neuron.

[10]  J. Pardo,et al.  Functional neuroimaging of the olfactory system in humans. , 2000, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[11]  N. Sobel,et al.  An odor is not worth a thousand words: from multidimensional odors to unidimensional odor objects. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.

[12]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Classical and Bayesian Inference in Neuroimaging: Applications , 2002, NeuroImage.

[14]  Fabian Grabenhorst,et al.  How Pleasant and Unpleasant Stimuli Combine in Different Brain Regions: Odor Mixtures , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Bruce R. Rosen,et al.  Activation and Habituation in Olfaction—An fMRI Study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[16]  J. Price,et al.  Central olfactory connections in the macaque monkey , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[17]  E T Rolls,et al.  Olfactory neuronal responses in the primate orbitofrontal cortex: analysis in an olfactory discrimination task. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Alexander J. Radnovich,et al.  Involvement of the left anterior insula and frontopolar gyrus in odor discrimination , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[19]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  Computational neuroscience of vision , 2002 .

[20]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of human working memory revisited Evidence from 3-T fMRI studies using classical domain-specific interference tasks , 2003, NeuroImage.

[21]  P. Goldman-Rakic The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[22]  Noam Sobel,et al.  Attentional modulation in human primary olfactory cortex , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  E. Rolls,et al.  Neurodynamics of biased competition and cooperation for attention: a model with spiking neurons. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  J. Price,et al.  Prefrontal cortical projections to the hypothalamus in Macaque monkeys , 1998, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[25]  B. Gulyás,et al.  Odorant differentiated pattern of cerebral activation: Comparison of acetone and vanillin , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[26]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Working Memory in Another Dimension: Functional Imaging of Human Olfactory Working Memory , 2001, NeuroImage.

[27]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  Warm pleasant feelings in the brain , 2008, NeuroImage.

[28]  Morten L. Kringelbach,et al.  Fast, Fully Automated Global and Local Magnetic Field Optimization for fMRI of the Human Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[29]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of working memory: , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[30]  T. Parrish,et al.  Learning to Smell the Roses: Experience-Dependent Neural Plasticity in Human Piriform and Orbitofrontal Cortices , 2006, Neuron.

[31]  S. Yaxley,et al.  Sensory-specific satiety: Food-specific reduction in responsiveness of ventral forebrain neurons after feeding in the monkey , 1986, Brain Research.

[32]  Thomas Hummel,et al.  Odor attributes change in relation to the time of the year. Cinnamon odor is more familiar and pleasant during Christmas season than summertime , 2009, Appetite.

[33]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of functional MRI time‐series , 1994, Human Brain Mapping.

[34]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Neural mechanisms involved in odor pleasantness and intensity judgments , 2000, Neuroreport.

[35]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[36]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex—An Update Time Is of the Essence , 2001, Neuron.

[37]  Hugo D. Critchley,et al.  The Representation of Information About Taste and Odor in the Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2010 .

[38]  E. Rolls,et al.  Attention, short-term memory, and action selection: A unifying theory , 2005, Progress in Neurobiology.

[39]  B. Gulyás,et al.  Smelling of Odorous Sex Hormone-like Compounds Causes Sex-Differentiated Hypothalamic Activations in Humans , 2001, Neuron.

[40]  E. Rolls,et al.  Representation of pleasant and aversive taste in the human brain. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Chantal Delon-Martin,et al.  fMRI of emotional responses to odors: influence of hedonic valence and judgment, handedness, and gender , 2003, NeuroImage.

[42]  E. Rolls,et al.  Hunger and satiety modify the responses of olfactory and visual neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Robust Smoothness Estimation in Statistical Parametric Maps Using Standardized Residuals from the General Linear Model , 1999, NeuroImage.

[44]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  Different representations of relative and absolute subjective value in the human brain , 2009, NeuroImage.

[45]  D. Small,et al.  Differential Neural Responses Evoked by Orthonasal versus Retronasal Odorant Perception in Humans , 2005, Neuron.

[46]  G. Mangun,et al.  Pre-target activity in visual cortex predicts behavioral performance on spatial and feature attention tasks , 2006, Brain Research.

[47]  E. Rolls,et al.  The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: From affect to decision-making , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.

[48]  D. Collins,et al.  Automatic 3D Intersubject Registration of MR Volumetric Data in Standardized Talairach Space , 1994, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[49]  E. Rolls,et al.  Orbitofrontal cortex neurons: role in olfactory and visual association learning. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[51]  E. Rolls,et al.  Different representations of pleasant and unpleasant odours in the human brain , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[52]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Longitudinal topography and interdigitation of corticostriatal projections in the rhesus monkey , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[53]  Norihiko Onoda,et al.  Olfactory input to the lateral hypothalamus of the old world monkey , 1987, Neuroscience Research.

[54]  James M Kilner,et al.  Integrated Neural Representations of Odor Intensity and Affective Valence in Human Amygdala , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  O. Gruber Effects of domain-specific interference on brain activation associated with verbal working memory task performance. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  E. Rolls,et al.  Representation of umami taste in the human brain. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.