Temporal evolution of neural activity and connectivity during microsleeps when rested and following sleep restriction

ABSTRACT Even when it is critical to stay awake, such as when driving, sleep deprivation weakens one's ability to do so by substantially increasing the propensity for microsleeps. Microsleeps are complete lapses of consciousness but, paradoxically, are associated with transient increases in cortical activity. But do microsleeps provide a benefit in terms of attenuating the need for sleep? And is the neural response to microsleeps altered by the degree of homeostatic drive to sleep? In this study, we continuously monitored eye‐video, visuomotor responsiveness, and brain activity via fMRI in 20 healthy subjects during a 20‐min visuomotor tracking task following a normally‐rested night and a sleep‐restricted (4‐h) night. As expected, sleep restriction led to an increased number of microsleeps and an increased variability in tracking error. Microsleeps exhibited transient increases in regional activity in the fronto‐parietal and parahippocampal area. Network analyses revealed divergent transient changes in the right fronto‐parietal, dorsal‐attention, default‐mode, and thalamo‐cortical functional networks. In all subjects, tracking error immediately following microsleeps was improved compared to before the microsleeps. Importantly, post‐microsleep recovery in tracking response speed was associated with hyperactivation in the thalamo‐cortical network. The temporal evolution of functional connectivity within the frontal and posterior nodes of the default‐mode network and between the right fronto‐parietal and default‐mode networks was associated with temporal changes in visuomotor responsiveness. These findings demonstrate distinct brain‐network‐level changes in brain activity during microsleeps and suggest that neural activity in the thalamo‐cortical network may facilitate the transient recovery from microsleeps. The temporal pattern of evolution in brain activity and performance is indicative of dynamic changes in vigilance during the struggle to stay awake following sleep loss. HIGHLIGHTSWe characterise performance and brain activity surrounding microsleeps.Microsleeps are associated with transient improvement in visuomotor performance.Brain networks show transient and tonic fMRI changes during microsleeps.Microsleeps reduce anticorrelations between default‐mode and executive networks.Microsleeps increase decoupling within the default‐mode network.

[1]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  M. Fukunaga,et al.  Low frequency BOLD fluctuations during resting wakefulness and light sleep: A simultaneous EEG‐fMRI study , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[3]  Michael W. L. Chee,et al.  Lapsing when sleep deprived: Neural activation characteristics of resistant and vulnerable individuals , 2010, NeuroImage.

[4]  D. Dinges,et al.  Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. , 2005, Seminars in neurology.

[5]  M. Chee,et al.  Lapsing during Sleep Deprivation Is Associated with Distributed Changes in Brain Activation , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  John A. Detre,et al.  Continuous ASL perfusion fMRI investigation of higher cognition: Quantification of tonic CBF changes during sustained attention and working memory tasks , 2006, NeuroImage.

[7]  Tzyy-Ping Jung,et al.  Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics in Continuous Compensatory Tracking Tasks , 2005, 2005 IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology 27th Annual Conference.

[8]  Juan Zhou,et al.  Spontaneous eyelid closures link vigilance fluctuation with fMRI dynamic connectivity states , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Richard D. Jones,et al.  Optimized echo state networks with leaky integrator neurons for EEG-based microsleep detection , 2015, 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC).

[10]  Govinda R. Poudel,et al.  Time-varying effective connectivity of the cortical neuroelectric activity associated with behavioural microsleeps , 2016, NeuroImage.

[11]  N. Volkow,et al.  Impairment of attentional networks after 1 night of sleep deprivation. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Reza Shoorangiz,et al.  Bayesian multi-subject factor analysis to predict microsleeps from EEG power spectral features , 2017, 2017 39th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC).

[13]  S. Doran,et al.  Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. , 2001, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[14]  M. Czisch,et al.  Development of the brain's default mode network from wakefulness to slow wave sleep. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  G. Tononi,et al.  Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. , 2006, Sleep medicine reviews.

[16]  Govinda R. Poudel,et al.  Microsleeps are Associated with Stage-2 Sleep Spindles from Hippocampal-Temporal Network , 2016, Int. J. Neural Syst..

[17]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[18]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference , 2009, NeuroImage.

[19]  Gregory G. Brown,et al.  Increased cerebral response during a divided attention task following sleep deprivation , 2001, Journal of sleep research.

[20]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[21]  Carrie R. H. Innes,et al.  Losing the struggle to stay awake: Divergent thalamic and cortical activity during microsleeps , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[22]  P. R. Davidson,et al.  Measurement of BOLD Changes Due to Cued Eye-Closure and Stopping During a Continuous Visuomotor Task via Model-Based and Model-Free Approaches , 2010, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.

[23]  Michael W. L. Chee,et al.  Sleep deprivation reduces default mode network connectivity and anti-correlation during rest and task performance , 2012, NeuroImage.

[24]  Carrie R. H. Innes,et al.  Efficient and Regular Patterns of Nighttime Sleep are Related to Increased Vulnerability to Microsleeps Following a Single Night of Sleep Restriction , 2013, Chronobiology international.

[25]  Richard D. Jones,et al.  Prediction of microsleeps using pairwise joint entropy and mutual information between EEG channels , 2017, 2017 39th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC).

[26]  Panagiotis Matsangas,et al.  The Epworth Sleepiness Scale in Service Members with Sleep Disorders. , 2019, Military medicine.

[27]  M. Johns,et al.  A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. , 1991, Sleep.

[28]  P. R. Davidson,et al.  Detection of lapses in responsiveness from the EEG , 2011, Journal of neural engineering.

[29]  Michael Brady,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[30]  G. Tononi,et al.  Local sleep in awake rats , 2011, Nature.

[31]  Carrie R. H. Innes,et al.  Cerebral perfusion differences between drowsy and nondrowsy individuals after acute sleep restriction. , 2012, Sleep.

[32]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Reza Shoorangiz,et al.  Prediction of microsleeps from EEG: Preliminary results , 2016, 2016 38th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC).

[34]  Mark Jenkinson,et al.  Fast, automated, N‐dimensional phase‐unwrapping algorithm , 2003, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[35]  Giulio Tononi,et al.  Local experience-dependent changes in the wake EEG after prolonged wakefulness. , 2013, Sleep.

[36]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. , 1990, The International journal of neuroscience.

[37]  P. R. Davidson,et al.  Frequent lapses of responsiveness during an extended visuomotor tracking task in non‐sleep‐deprived subjects , 2006, Journal of sleep research.

[38]  Róbert Bódizs,et al.  Wakefulness–sleep transition: Emerging electroencephalographic similarities with the rapid eye movement phase , 2008, Brain Research Bulletin.

[39]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  M. Rizzo,et al.  Driver Performance in the Moments Surrounding a Microsleep. , 2008, Transportation research. Part F, Traffic psychology and behaviour.

[41]  A. Zalesky,et al.  Competitive and cooperative dynamics of large-scale brain functional networks supporting recollection , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  Richard D. Jones,et al.  EEG-Based Lapse Detection With High Temporal Resolution , 2007, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[43]  M. J. Meloy,et al.  The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. , 2005, Sleep.

[44]  B. T. Thomas Yeo,et al.  Co-activated yet disconnected—Neural correlates of eye closures when trying to stay awake , 2015, NeuroImage.