Effects of Search Difficulty on the Selection, Maintenance, and Learning of Attentional Templates

Prominent theories of attention claim that visual search is guided through attentional templates stored in working memory. Recently, the contralateral delay activity (CDA), an electrophysiological index of working memory storage, has been found to rapidly decrease when participants repeatedly search for the same target, suggesting that, with learning, the template moves out of working memory. However, this has only been investigated with pop-out search for distinct targets, for which a strong attentional template may not be necessary. More effortful search tasks might rely more on an active attentional template in working memory, leading to a slower handoff to long-term memory and thus a slower decline of the CDA. Using ERPs, we compared the rate of learning of attentional templates in pop-out and effortful search tasks. In two experiments, the rate of decrease in the CDA was the same for both search tasks. Similar results were found for a second component indexing working memory effort, the late positive complex. However, the late positive complex was also sensitive to anticipated search difficulty, as was expressed in a greater amplitude before the harder search task. We conclude that the amount of working memory effort invested in maintaining an attentional template, but not the rate of learning, depends on search difficulty.

[1]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .

[2]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  C. C. Wood,et al.  ERPs predictive of subsequent recall and recognition performance , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[4]  Christian N. L. Olivers,et al.  Intertrial priming stemming from ambiguity: A new account of priming in visual search , 2006 .

[5]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  R. Desimone,et al.  Responses of Neurons in Inferior Temporal Cortex during Memory- Guided Visual Search , 1998 .

[7]  A. A. Wijers,et al.  An event-related brain potential correlate of visual short-term memory. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[8]  S. Luck,et al.  The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components , 2011 .

[9]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[10]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[11]  A Gevins,et al.  Dynamic cortical networks of verbal and spatial working memory: effects of memory load and task practice. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  P. Berg,et al.  Ocular artifacts in EEG and event-related potentials I: Scalp topography , 2005, Brain Topography.

[13]  E. Donchin,et al.  P300 and tracking difficulty: evidence for multiple resources in dual-task performance. , 1980, Psychophysiology.

[14]  A. Treisman,et al.  Search asymmetry: a diagnostic for preattentive processing of separable features. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  A Kok,et al.  Event-related potentials to conjunctions of spatial frequency and orientation as a function of stimulus parameters and response requirements. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2004, Nature.

[17]  Jason T. Arita,et al.  Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory , 2011, Psychological science.

[18]  G. Potts An ERP index of task relevance evaluation of visual stimuli , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[19]  J. Tecce Contingent negative variation (CNV) and psychological processes in man. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  Terence W. Picton,et al.  Ocular artifacts in recording EEGs and event-related potentials II: Source dipoles and source components , 2005, Brain Topography.

[21]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  Electrophysiological Correlates of the Focusing of Attention within Complex Visual Scenes: N2pc and Related ERP Components , 2011 .

[22]  C. C. Wood,et al.  The ɛ-Adjustment Procedure for Repeated-Measures Analyses of Variance , 1976 .

[23]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[24]  J. Leon Kenemans,et al.  Selective attention to spatial frequency: an ERP and source localization analysis , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[25]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  C D Wickens,et al.  Processing of stimulus properties: evidence for dual-task integrality. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  J. Polich Neuropsychology of P300 , 2011 .

[28]  Nancy B. Carlisle,et al.  Where do we store the memory representations that guide attention? , 2013, Journal of vision.

[29]  Vincent Di Lollo,et al.  Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  D. Ruchkin,et al.  Short-term memory storage and retention: an event-related brain potential study. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[31]  C. Bundesen,et al.  A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology. , 2005, Psychological review.

[32]  M. Stokes ‘Activity-silent’ working memory in prefrontal cortex: a dynamic coding framework , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  E. Vogel,et al.  Contralateral delay activity provides a neural measure of the number of representations in visual working memory. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  A. Kok On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[35]  Daniel S. Ruchkin,et al.  The Late Positive Complex , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[36]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[37]  Geoffrey F Woodman,et al.  Serial deployment of attention during visual search. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  Geoffrey F. Woodman,et al.  Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Tzyy-Ping Jung,et al.  Independent Component Analysis of Electroencephalographic Data , 1995, NIPS.

[40]  M. Eimer The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[41]  D. Tucker,et al.  Frontal evaluation and posterior representation in target detection. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[42]  Eren Gunseli,et al.  Is a search template an ordinary working memory? Comparing electrophysiological markers of working memory maintenance for visual search and recognition , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  K Grune,et al.  Updating of working memory in a running memory task: an event-related potential study. , 2000, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[44]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[45]  Ken A. Paller,et al.  Finding meaning in novel geometric shapes influences electrophysiological correlates of repetition and dissociates perceptual and conceptual priming , 2010, NeuroImage.

[46]  R. Rosenthal,et al.  Contrast Analysis: Focused Comparisons in the Analysis of Variance , 1985 .

[47]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Electrophysiological Measures of Maintaining Representations in Visual Working Memory , 2007, Cortex.

[48]  L. Garcia-Larrea,et al.  P3, positive slow wave and working memory load: a study on the functional correlates of slow wave activity. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[49]  J. Theeuwes Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[50]  John Duncan,et al.  A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex , 1993, Nature.

[51]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.