Video games: A significant cognitive artifact of contemporary youth culture

Video games are not just an important cultural artifact of youth culture but have considerable cognitive worth. Centered within an information processing theory and mediating processes' framework, the empirical qualitative study investigated, via stimulated recall methods, the thinking skills and strategies of five teenagers while playing an action-adventure video game. Sixteen types and 600 instances of cognitive skills and 11 types and 155 instances of cognitive strategies were identified. The thinking skills included high engagement with school valued cognitive skills, such as metacognition, and deduction and induction strategies. The findings support the informal educative value of playing recreation video games and their inclusion in schools.

[1]  M T Reinitz,et al.  A demonstration and comparison of two types of inference-based memory errors. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[2]  Richard C. Atkinson,et al.  Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes , 1968, Psychology of Learning and Motivation.

[3]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Cognitive socialization by computer games in two cultures: inductive discovery or mastery of an iconic code , 1994 .

[4]  John Kirriemuir,et al.  Literature Review in Games and Learning , 2004 .

[5]  R. Almond,et al.  Focus Article: On the Structure of Educational Assessments , 2003 .

[6]  Lloyd P. Rieber,et al.  Seriously considering play: Designing interactive learning environments based on the blending of microworlds, simulations, and games , 1996 .

[7]  A. Stapleton,et al.  Why video games are cool and school sucks , 2003 .

[8]  P. Vermersch Introspection as Practice , 2022 .

[9]  S. Keller Children and the Nintendo. , 1992 .

[10]  Patricia M. Greenfield,et al.  Two-dimensional representation of movement through three- dimensional space: The role of video game expertise☆ , 1994 .

[11]  P. Pintrich,et al.  Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. , 1990 .

[12]  Hitendra K. Pillay,et al.  Cognition and Recreational Computer Games: Implications for Educational Technology , 1999 .

[13]  Russell G. Almond,et al.  On the Structure of Educational Assessments, CSE Technical Report. , 2003 .

[14]  Complative,et al.  CENTER FOR THE STUDY OF EVALUATION , 2007 .

[15]  S. Gass,et al.  Stimulated Recall Methodology in Second Language Research , 2000 .

[16]  Marilyn McMeniman,et al.  Stimulated recall — An effective methodology for examining successful teaching in science , 1992 .

[17]  Evelyn C. Ferstl Learning from Text , 2001 .

[18]  W. Kintsch Learning From Text , 1986, Knowing, Learning, and Instruction.

[19]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Verbal reports as data. , 1980 .

[20]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Pro-cessing: A Framework for Memory Research , 1975 .

[21]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[22]  James Paul Gee,et al.  What video games have to teach us about learning and literacy , 2007, CIE.

[23]  L. Reder Plausibility Judgments versus Fact Retrieval: Alternative Strategies for Sentence Verification. , 1982 .

[24]  Chin-Chung Tsai,et al.  Student Computer Achievement, Attitude, and Anxiety: The Role of Learning Strategies , 2003 .

[25]  W. Doyle Paradigms in Teacher Effectiveness Research. , 1975 .

[26]  A. Nicolopoulou,et al.  Play, Cognitive Development, and the Social World: Piaget, Vygotsky, and Beyond , 1993 .

[27]  M. Scheerer,et al.  Problem Solving , 1967, Nature.

[28]  S. J. French,et al.  Accent on teaching : experiments in general education , 1954 .

[29]  D. Halpern Thought and Knowledge: An Introduction to Critical Thinking , 1995 .