Indirect Prime-to-Behavior Effects: The Role of Perceptions of the Self, Others, and Situations in Connecting Primed Constructs to Social Behavior

For more than a decade, researchers have convincingly shown that people's social behavior can be affected by primed constructs without people having any awareness of their influence. Earlier research proposed direct priming accounts for these effects, suggesting that primed constructs exert their effect on behavior in a relatively direct fashion without an intervening role for perceptual processes. In this chapter, we review evidence in favor of an indirect priming account for behavioral priming effects. In these indirect priming effects, a primed construct affects behavior via shifts in perceptions of a perceptual target. We review three types of indirect priming mechanisms: a self-perception, person-perception, and situation-perception mechanism. We also present various moderators that affect the direction and magnitude of each of the indirect priming effects. In addition, we identify factors, related to the attentional focus of the prime recipient, that indicate when each of the different mechanisms operates. Understanding the role of perceptual processes in the prime-to-behavior pathway can unravel more mysteries about the rich and complex nature of social behavior.

[1]  L. Berkowitz,et al.  Some effects of thoughts on anti- and prosocial influences of media events: a cognitive-neoassociation analysis. , 1984, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  Kenneth G. DeMarree,et al.  The Roles of the Self in Priming-to-Behavior Effects. , 2005 .

[3]  Yuwei Jiang,et al.  The Unique Consequences of Feeling Lucky: Implications for Consumer Behavior , 2009 .

[4]  Kenneth G. DeMarree,et al.  Who am I and who are you? Priming and the influence of self versus other focused attention , 2009 .

[5]  D. Stapel,et al.  It depends on your perspective: The role of self-relevance in stereotype-based underperformance , 2006 .

[6]  J. Bargh The automaticity of everyday life. , 1997 .

[7]  Amy N. Dalton,et al.  Nonconscious relationship reactance: When significant others prime opposing goals , 2007 .

[8]  J. Bargh,et al.  The perception–behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. , 2001 .

[9]  R. E. Burnkrant,et al.  A modification of the Fenigstein, Scheier, and Buss Self-Consciousness Scales. , 1984, Journal of personality assessment.

[10]  P. Devine Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. , 1989 .

[11]  A. Dijksterhuis,et al.  Effect of Social Category Priming on Personal Attitudes , 2003, Psychological science.

[12]  S. Milgram BEHAVIORAL STUDY OF OBEDIENCE. , 1963, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[13]  Nira Liberman,et al.  Seven Principles of Goal Activation: A Systematic Approach to Distinguishing Goal Priming From Priming of Non-Goal Constructs , 2007, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[14]  Gillian Ku,et al.  Perspective-takers behave more stereotypically. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  J. G. Holmes,et al.  An Atlas of Interpersonal Situations , 2003 .

[16]  Lysann Damisch,et al.  Going back to Donald: how comparisons shape judgmental priming effects. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  George Y. Bizer,et al.  Self‐Schema Matching and Attitude Change: Situational and Dispositional Determinants of Message Elaboration , 2005 .

[18]  Kimberly Rios Morrison,et al.  Significant Other Primes and Behavior: Motivation to Respond to Social Cues Moderates Pursuit of Prime-Induced Goals , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[19]  L. Warlop,et al.  When do primes prime? The moderating role of the self-concept in individuals' susceptibility to priming effects on social behavior , 2009 .

[20]  S. C. Wheeler,et al.  The “Shaken Self”: Product Choices as a Means of Restoring Self‐View Confidence , 2009 .

[21]  S. Balasubramanian,et al.  Testing the dimensionality of the self-consciousness scales. , 1987, Journal of personality assessment.

[22]  Jerry Suls,et al.  Method matters: effects of explicit versus implicit social comparisons on activation, behavior, and self-views. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  James Andreoni,et al.  What Do Bargainers' Preferences Look Like? Experiments with a Convex Ultimatum Game , 2003 .

[24]  M. Ferguson,et al.  Beyond behaviorism: on the automaticity of higher mental processes. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  A. Dijksterhuis,et al.  Behavioral indecision : Effects of self-focus on automatic behavior , 2000 .

[26]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  DISPOSITIONAL DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE MOTIVATION : THE LIFE AND TIMES OF INDIVIDUALS VARYING IN NEED FOR COGNITION , 1996 .

[27]  R. Spears,et al.  Reaction in Action: Intergroup Contrast in Automatic Behavior , 2004, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[28]  D. Stapel In the Mind of the Beholder: The Interpretation Comparison Model of Accessibility Effects. , 2007 .

[29]  D. Stapel,et al.  The self salience model of other-to-self effects: integrating principles of self-enhancement, complementarity, and imitation. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  A theory of goal systems. , 2002 .

[31]  P. V. Lange,et al.  The pursuit of joint outcomes and equality in outcomes: An integrative model of social value orientation. , 1999 .

[32]  M. Banaji,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Processes in Stereotype Priming , 1996 .

[33]  D. Messick,et al.  Motivational bases of choice in experimental games , 1968 .

[34]  A. D. Jones,et al.  Obedience to Authority , 1974 .

[35]  Thomas E. Nelson,et al.  Stereotypes and social judgment: Extremity, assimilation, and contrast. , 1988 .

[36]  I. Ajzen The theory of planned behavior , 1991 .

[37]  Paul A. M. Van Lange,et al.  Social Value Orientations and Impressions of Partner's Honesty and Intelligence: A Test of the Might Versus Morality Effect , 1994 .

[38]  L. Putterman,et al.  Reciprocity in a two-part dictator game , 2004 .

[39]  R. Petty,et al.  The effects of stereotype activation on behavior: a review of possible mechanisms. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  G. Moskowitz,et al.  The Implicit Volition Model: On the Preconscious Regulation of Temporarily Adopted Goals , 2004 .

[41]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  Thomas W. Schubert,et al.  Contrast from social stereotypes in automatic behavior , 2003 .

[43]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  HELP, I NEED SOMEBODY: AUTOMATIC ACTION AND INACTION , 1998 .

[44]  Sonja Utz,et al.  Self-Construal and Cooperation: Is the Interdependent Self More Cooperative Than the Independent Self? , 2004 .

[45]  P. Herr,et al.  Consequences of priming: Judgment and behavior. , 1986 .

[46]  Paul A. M. Van Lange,et al.  Social value orientation and intelligence: A test of the goal prescribes rationality principle , 1991 .

[47]  Leif D. Nelson,et al.  From Student to Superhero: Situational Primes Shape Future Helping , 2003 .

[48]  K. Sassenberg,et al.  Knowing how to react: automatic response priming from social categories. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  D. Krull,et al.  Does the Grist Change the Mill? The Effect of the Perceiver's Inferential Goal on the Process of Social Inference , 1993 .

[50]  Paul A. M. Van Lange,et al.  Impression formation and cooperative behavior , 1999 .

[51]  Robyn A. Leboeuf,et al.  "FORTUNATELY, I'M NO EINSTEIN": COMPARISON RELEVANCE AS A DETERMINANT OF BEHAVIORAL ASSIMILATION AND CONTRAST , 2004 .

[52]  K. Lashley The problem of serial order in behavior , 1951 .

[53]  T. Meyer,et al.  Blonde Like Me: When Self-Construals Moderate Stereotype Priming Effects on Intellectual Performance , 2008 .

[54]  Guido Hertel,et al.  Priming in-group favoritism: The impact of normative scripts in the minimal group paradigm. , 2001 .

[55]  Kenneth G. DeMarree,et al.  A match made in the laboratory: Persuasion and matches to primed traits and stereotypes , 2008 .

[56]  W. Güth,et al.  An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining , 1982 .

[57]  Stephen C. Jones,et al.  The self-fulfilling prophecy and interpersonal attraction , 1971 .

[58]  M. Wänke,et al.  Liking What's FamiLiar: the importance oF UnconscioUs FamiLiarity in the mere-exposUre eFFect , 2009 .

[59]  W. Liebrand,et al.  The Influence of Other's Morality and Own Social Value Orientation on Cooperation in the Netherlands and the U.S.A. , 1991 .

[60]  H. Aarts,et al.  The silence of the library: environment, situational norm, and social behavior. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[61]  G. Hertel,et al.  What do you think is "fair"? Effects of ingroup norms and outcome control on fairness judgments , 2002 .

[62]  J. Shah,et al.  Automatic for the people: how representations of significant others implicitly affect goal pursuit. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[63]  A. van Knippenberg,et al.  The relation between perception and behavior, or how to win a game of trivial pursuit. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[64]  H. Markus,et al.  THE DYNAMIC SELF-CONCEPT: A Social Psychological Perspective , 1987 .

[65]  G. Maio,et al.  The Psychology of Attitudes and Attitude Change , 2015 .

[66]  Gráinne M. Fitzsimons,et al.  Thinking of you: nonconscious pursuit of interpersonal goals associated with relationship partners. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[67]  J. G. Holmes Interpersonal expectations as the building blocks of social cognition: An interdependence theory perspective. , 2002 .

[68]  Kenneth G. DeMarree,et al.  Multiple Mechanisms of Prime‐to‐Behavior Effects , 2009 .

[69]  K. Fiedler,et al.  Fair and dependent versus egoistic and free: effects of semantic and evaluative priming on the ‘Ring Measure of Social Values’ , 1998 .

[70]  Katie A. Liljenquist,et al.  Washing Away Your Sins: Threatened Morality and Physical Cleansing , 2006, Science.

[71]  J. Bargh,et al.  Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[72]  L. Tiedens,et al.  Power moves: complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[73]  H. Aarts On the Emergence of Human Goal Pursuit: The Nonconscious Regulation and Motivation of Goals , 2007 .

[74]  M. Zanna,et al.  Let's not be indifferent about (attitudinal) ambivalence. , 1995 .

[75]  E. Higgins,et al.  Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. , 1987, Psychological review.

[76]  N. Mandel,et al.  The Sweet Escape: Effects of Mortality Salience on Consumption Quantities for High- and Low-Self-Esteem Consumers , 2008 .

[77]  Kenneth G. DeBono,et al.  Cognitive dissonance and self-monitoring: A matter of context? , 1989 .

[78]  B. Latané The psychology of social impact. , 1981 .

[79]  Kenneth G. DeMarree,et al.  Priming a new identity: self-monitoring moderates the effects of nonself primes on self-judgments and behavior. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[80]  E. Higgins Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. , 1996 .

[81]  Sonja Utz Self-activation is a two-edged sword: The effects of I primes on cooperation , 2004 .

[82]  W. Prinz A common-coding approach to perception and action , 1990 .

[83]  Mark Snyder,et al.  Behavioral confirmation in social interaction: From social perception to social reality. , 1978 .

[84]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Chapter 5 Human Mimicry , 2009 .

[85]  Kenneth G. DeMarree,et al.  Self-Ambivalence and Resistance to Subtle Self-Change Attempts , 2011, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[86]  Kenneth G. DeMarree,et al.  Self-certainty: Parallels to Attitude Certainty , 2007 .

[87]  S. Fiske,et al.  Personality feedback and situational norms can control stereotyping processes. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[88]  T. Mussweiler Comparison processes in social judgment: mechanisms and consequences. , 2003, Psychological review.

[89]  H. Kelley Attribution in social interaction. , 1987 .

[90]  J. Horowitz,et al.  Fairness in Simple Bargaining Experiments , 1994 .

[91]  S. Christian Wheeler,et al.  Circles, Squares, and Choice: The Effect of Shape Arrays on Uniqueness and Variety Seeking , 2008 .

[92]  Joseph Cesario,et al.  Automatic social behavior as motivated preparation to interact. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[93]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Automatic Activation of Impression Formation and Memorization Goals: Nonconscious Goal Priming Reproduces Effects of Explicit Task Instructions , 1996 .

[94]  J. Bargh What have we been priming all these years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. , 2006, European journal of social psychology.

[95]  J. Geanakoplos,et al.  Psychological games and sequential rationality , 1989 .

[96]  P. V. van Lange,et al.  On perceiving morality and potency: Social values and the effects of person perception in a give‐some dilemma , 1989 .

[97]  Thomas E. Nelson,et al.  Stereotypes and Standards of Judgment , 1991 .

[98]  C. Carver,et al.  Modeling: An analysis in terms of category accessibility. , 1983 .

[99]  C. Sedikides Attentional Effects on Mood are Moderated by Chronic Self-Conception Valence , 1992 .

[100]  K. Markman,et al.  A Reflection and Evaluation Model of Comparative Thinking , 2003, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[101]  J. Bargh,et al.  The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[102]  Tom Postmes,et al.  Seeing one thing and doing another: Contrast effects in automatic behavior. , 1998 .

[103]  C. Rusbult,et al.  Interdependence, interaction, and relationships. , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[104]  S. C. Wheeler,et al.  Contextual priming: Where people vote affects how they vote , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[105]  Roderick M. Kramer,et al.  Equity and Social Exchange in Human Relationships , 1984 .

[106]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The need for cognition. , 1982 .

[107]  Aaron C. Kay,et al.  Material priming: The influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal and competitive behavioral choice , 2004 .

[108]  Aaron C. Kay,et al.  The role of interpersonal perceptions in the prime-to-behavior pathway. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[109]  H. Aarts,et al.  Goal-Discrepant Situations Prime Goal-Directed Actions if Goals Are Temporarily or Chronically Accessible , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[110]  H. Kelley,et al.  Social interaction basis of cooperators' and competitors' beliefs about others. , 1970 .

[111]  D. Smeesters,et al.  Knowledge activation after information encoding: Implications of trait priming on person judgment , 2008 .

[112]  K. Kaplan On the ambivalence-indifference problem in attitude theory and measurement: A suggested modification of the semantic differential technique. , 1972 .

[113]  J. Piliavin,et al.  What is the Factorial Structure of the Private and Public Self-Consciousness Scales? , 1988 .

[114]  T. Mussweiler,et al.  The man who wasn't there: Subliminal social comparison standards influence self-evaluation ☆ , 2004 .

[115]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Category accessibility and social perception: Some implications for the study of person memory and interpersonal judgments , 1980 .

[116]  Olivier Corneille,et al.  Do not prime hawks with doves: the interplay of construct activation and consistency of social value orientation on cooperative behavior. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[117]  Kenneth G. DeMarree,et al.  Understanding the Role of the Self in Prime-to-Behavior Effects: The Active-Self Account , 2007, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[118]  Charles G. McClintock,et al.  Social motivation: A set of propositions. , 1972 .

[119]  Gráinne M. Fitzsimons,et al.  Automatic Effects of Brand Exposure on Motivated Behavior: How Apple Makes You "Think Different" , 2008 .

[120]  E. Tanke,et al.  Behavior and attitude: Some people are more consistent than others. , 1976 .

[121]  Richard E. Petty,et al.  Think Unto Others: The Self-Destructive Impact of Negative Racial Stereotypes ☆ ☆☆ , 2001 .

[122]  J. Bargh,et al.  Automatic information processing and social perception: The influence of trait information presented outside of conscious awareness on impression formation. , 1982 .

[123]  V. Jefferis,et al.  The influence of social groups on goal contagion , 2008 .

[124]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Perceiving an Object and Its Context in Different Cultures , 2003, Psychological science.

[125]  J. Pligt,et al.  The Referents of Trait Inferences: The Impact of Trait Concepts Versus Actor-Trait Links on Subsequent Judgments , 1996 .

[126]  L. Slone,et al.  The nonconsciousness of self-consciousness. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[127]  M. Clark,et al.  Interest in Another's Consideration of One's Needs in Communal and Exchange Relationships☆☆☆★ , 1998 .

[128]  Rumen Iliev,et al.  Sinning Saints and Saintly Sinners , 2009, Psychological science.

[129]  J. Bargh,et al.  On the Relation between Associative Strength and Automatic Behavior , 2000 .

[130]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  Priming us and them: automatic assimilation and contrast in group attitudes. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[131]  R. Crisp,et al.  Assimilation and contrast to group primes: * : The moderating role of ingroup identification , 2008 .

[132]  W. Carpenter Principles of Mental Physiology , 1874, Nature.

[133]  M. Scheier,et al.  Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. , 1975 .

[134]  D. Stapel,et al.  I, we, and the effects of others on me: How self-construal level moderates social comparison effects. , 2001 .

[135]  H. Markus,et al.  Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. , 1991 .

[136]  D. Bem Self-Perception Theory , 1972 .

[137]  D. M. Kuhlman,et al.  Expectations of choice behavior held by cooperators, competitors, and individualists across four classes of experimental games. , 1976 .

[138]  W. S. Rholes,et al.  Category accessibility and impression formation , 1977 .

[139]  L. Bohon,et al.  Factor structure of the Private Self-Consciousness Scale. , 1996, Journal of personality assessment.

[140]  Mark P. Zanna,et al.  The Nonverbal Mediation of Self-Fulfilling Prophecies in Interracial Interaction. , 1974 .

[141]  J. Bargh,et al.  Automaticity in action: The unconscious as repository of chronic goals and motives. , 1996 .

[142]  H. Markus,et al.  Stability and malleability of the self-concept. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[143]  M. Snyder Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. , 1974 .

[144]  L. Ross,et al.  The Name of the Game: Predictive Power of Reputations versus Situational Labels in Determining Prisoner’s Dilemma Game Moves , 2004, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[145]  Kenneth G. DeMarree,et al.  Self and Attitude Strength Parallels: Focus on Accessibility , 2007 .

[146]  E. Bruin,et al.  The double meaning of a single act: influences of the perceiver and the perceived on cooperative behaviour , 1999 .

[147]  D. Gilbert,et al.  The correspondence bias. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[148]  James C. Hamilton,et al.  Self-awareness mediates the relationship between serial position and item reliability , 1990 .

[149]  M. Rabin Published by: American , 2022 .

[150]  Carl A. Kallgren,et al.  A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. , 1990 .

[151]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  Constructing reality and its alternatives: an inclusion/ exclusion model of assimilation and contrast effects in social judgment , 1992 .

[152]  Thomas Gilovich,et al.  Just Going Along: Nonconscious Priming and Conformity to Social Pressure , 1999 .

[153]  L. Tiedens,et al.  Assimilation for affiliation and contrast for control: complementary self-construals. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[154]  K. Fiedler,et al.  Affective and cognitive influences in social dilemma game , 1994 .

[155]  S. Utz,et al.  What is smart in a social dilemma? differential effects of priming competence on cooperation , 2004 .

[156]  Aaron C. Kay,et al.  The situated person: Effects of construct accessibility on situation construals and interpersonal perception , 2008 .

[157]  P. Herr,et al.  On the consequences of priming: Assimilation and contrast effects , 1983 .

[158]  W. N. Schoenfeld,et al.  Principles of Psychology , 2007 .

[159]  M. Arbib,et al.  Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[160]  D. Christopher Dryer,et al.  When Do Opposites Attract? Interpersonal Complementarity Versus Similarity , 1997 .

[161]  Kenneth G. DeMarree,et al.  Does self-consciousness increase or decrease priming effects? It depends , 2008 .

[162]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[163]  Jonah Berger,et al.  When the Same Prime Leads to Different Effects , 2007 .

[164]  Sandra J. Milberg,et al.  Recipient's Mood, Relationship Type, and Helping , 1987 .

[165]  T. K. Srull,et al.  The Role of Category Accessibility in the Interpretation of Information About Persons: Some Determinants and Implications , 1979 .

[166]  M. Conner,et al.  Social cognition models and health behaviour: A structured review , 2000 .

[167]  M. Ferguson,et al.  Taking a closer look: On the operation of nonconscious impression formation. , 2008, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[168]  Wim B. G. Liebrand,et al.  The effect of social motives, communication and group size on behaviour in an N-person multi-stage mixed-motive game , 1984 .

[169]  H. Aarts,et al.  Positive affect as implicit motivator: on the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[170]  Aaron C. Kay,et al.  The perceptual push: The interplay of implicit cues and explicit situational construals on behavioral intentions in the Prisoner's Dilemma , 2003 .

[171]  S. Duval,et al.  Opinion change and performance facilitation as a result of objective self-awareness ☆ , 1971 .

[172]  H. Aarts,et al.  Habits as knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior , 2000 .

[173]  Ran R. Hassin,et al.  Goal contagion: perceiving is for pursuing. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[174]  Joop van der Pligt,et al.  Categories of Category Accessibility: The Impact of Trait Concept versus Exemplar Priming on Person Judgments ☆ , 1997 .

[175]  Steven L. Neuberg,et al.  Behavioral Implications of Information Presented Outside of Conscious Awareness: The Effect of Subliminal Presentation of Trait Information on Behavior in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game , 1988 .

[176]  H. Markus,et al.  The Willful Self , 1990 .

[177]  S. Duval,et al.  A theory of objective self awareness , 1972 .