The Spatial Scale of Social Learning Affects Cultural Diversity

Sewall Wright's (1943) concept of isolation by distance is as germane to cultural transmission as genetic transmission. Yet there has been little research on how the spatial scale of social learning—the geographic extent of cultural transmission—affects cultural diversity. Here, we employ agent-based simulation to study how the spatial scale of unbiased social learning affects selectively neutral cultural diversity over a range of population sizes and densities. We show that highly localized unbiased cultural transmission may be easily confused with a form of biased cultural transmission, especially in low-density populations. Our results have important implications for how archaeologists infer mechanisms of cultural transmission from diversity estimates that depart from the expectations of neutral theory.

[1]  S. Wright Evolution in mendelian populations , 1931 .

[2]  Sewall Wright,et al.  Breeding Structure of Populations in Relation to Speciation , 1940, The American Naturalist.

[3]  S. Wright,et al.  Isolation by Distance. , 1943, Genetics.

[4]  S. Wright,et al.  Isolation by distance under diverse systems of mating. , 1946, Genetics.

[5]  M. Kimura,et al.  An introduction to population genetics theory , 1971 .

[6]  W. Ewens The sampling theory of selectively neutral alleles. , 1972, Theoretical population biology.

[7]  W. Ewens Mathematical Population Genetics , 1980 .

[8]  R. Bishop,et al.  Ceramic Compositional Analysis in Archaeological Perspective , 1980 .

[9]  M. Feldman,et al.  Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. , 1981, Monographs in population biology.

[10]  P. Richerson,et al.  Culture and the Evolutionary Process , 1988 .

[11]  M Slatkin,et al.  An exact test for neutrality based on the Ewens sampling distribution. , 1994, Genetical research.

[12]  F. Neiman Stylistic Variation in Evolutionary Perspective: Inferences from Decorative Diversity and Interassemblage Distance in Illinois Woodland Ceramic Assemblages , 1995, American Antiquity.

[13]  M Slatkin,et al.  A correction to the exact test based on the Ewens sampling distribution. , 1996, Genetical research.

[14]  Timothy A. Kohler,et al.  Dynamics in human and primate societies: agent-based modeling of social and spatial processes , 2000 .

[15]  S. Shennan,et al.  Ceramic Style Change and Neutral Evolution: A Case Study from Neolithic Europe , 2001, American Antiquity.

[16]  P. Jeffrey Brantingham,et al.  A Neutral Model of Stone Raw Material Procurement , 2003, American Antiquity.

[17]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of cultural evolution , 2003 .

[18]  Timothy A. Kohler,et al.  Vessels and villages: evidence for conformist transmission in early village aggregations on the Pajarito Plateau, New Mexico , 2004 .

[19]  L. Premo,et al.  PATCHINESS AND PROSOCIALITY: MODELING THE EVOLUTION AND ARCHAEOLOGY OF PLIO-PLEISTOCENE HOMININ FOOD SHARING , 2006 .

[20]  Herawati Sudoyo,et al.  Male dominance rarely skews the frequency distribution of Y chromosome haplotypes in human populations , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Luke S. Premo Exploring behavioral terra incognita with archaeological agentbased models , 2008 .