Quantitative relationships in delphinid neocortex

Possessing large brains and complex behavioral patterns, cetaceans are believed to be highly intelligent. Their brains, which are the largest in the Animal Kingdom and have enormous gyrification compared with terrestrial mammals, have long been of scientific interest. Few studies, however, report total number of brain cells in cetaceans, and even fewer have used unbiased counting methods. In this study, using stereological methods, we estimated the total number of cells in the neocortex of the long-finned pilot whale (Globicephala melas) brain. For the first time, we show that a species of dolphin has more neocortical neurons than any mammal studied to date including humans. These cell numbers are compared across various mammals with different brain sizes, and the function of possessing many neurons is discussed. We found that the long-finned pilot whale neocortex has approximately 37.2 × 109 neurons, which is almost twice as many as humans, and 127 × 109 glial cells. Thus, the absolute number of neurons in the human neocortex is not correlated with the superior cognitive abilities of humans (at least compared to cetaceans) as has previously been hypothesized. However, as neuron density in long-finned pilot whales is lower than that in humans, their higher cell number appears to be due to their larger brain. Accordingly, our findings make an important contribution to the ongoing debate over quantitative relationships in the mammalian brain.

[1]  P. Morgane,et al.  Sensory Neocortex in Dolphin Brain , 1990 .

[2]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Culture in whales and dolphins , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[3]  D. Dubowitz,et al.  Functional imaging of dolphin brain metabolism and blood flow , 2006, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[4]  S. Ridgway,et al.  Neuronal Fiber Composition of the Corpus Callosum Within Some Odontocetes , 2008, Anatomical record.

[5]  H. J. G. GUNDERSEN,et al.  Some new, simple and efficient stereological methods and their use in pathological research and diagnosis , 1988, APMIS : acta pathologica, microbiologica, et immunologica Scandinavica.

[6]  P. Morgane,et al.  Implications of the “initial brain” concept for brain evolution in Cetacea , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[7]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Behavioural evidence for social units in long-finned pilot whales , 2003 .

[8]  Bente Pakkenberg,et al.  Total numbers of neurons and glial cells in cortex and basal ganglia of aged brains with Down syndrome--a stereological study. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Diana Reiss,et al.  Self-recognition in an Asian elephant , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  R. Dietz,et al.  Diving behaviour of long-finned pilot whales Globicephala melas around the Faroe Islands , 2002, Wildlife Biology.

[11]  Jon H. Kaas,et al.  Gorilla and Orangutan Brains Conform to the Primate Cellular Scaling Rules: Implications for Human Evolution , 2011, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[12]  B. Pakkenberg,et al.  Total neocortical cell number in the mysticete brain , 2007, Anatomical record.

[13]  P. Hof,et al.  Evolution of increased glia–neuron ratios in the human frontal cortex , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  P. Hof,et al.  Animal Studies Repository Animal Studies Repository Cortical Complexity in Cetacean Brains , 2022 .

[15]  P.,et al.  Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals , 2017 .

[16]  H. Gundersen,et al.  Unbiased stereological estimation of the total number of neurons in the subdivisions of the rat hippocampus using the optical fractionator , 1991, The Anatomical record.

[17]  P. Morgane,et al.  Ultrastructure of synapses and golgi analysis of neurons in neocortex of the lateral gyrus (visual cortex) of the dolphin and pilot whale , 1990, Brain Research Bulletin.

[18]  Osama B. Mohammed,et al.  In contrast to many other mammals, cetaceans have relatively small hippocampi that appear to lack adult neurogenesis , 2013, Brain Structure and Function.

[19]  Myron S. Jacobs,et al.  The anatomy of the brain of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Surface configurations of the telencephalon of the bottlenose dolphin with comparative anatomical observations in four other cetacean species , 1980, Brain Research Bulletin.

[20]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Cetaceans Have Complex Brains for Complex Cognition , 2007, PLoS biology.

[21]  J. Mann,et al.  Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  P. Morgane,et al.  Calcium-binding protein-containing neuronal populations in mammalian visual cortex: a comparative study in whales, insectivores, bats, rodents, and primates. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  S. Herculano‐Houzel Brains matter, bodies maybe not: the case for examining neuron numbers irrespective of body size , 2011, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  J. Siegel,et al.  Unihemispheric slow wave sleep and the state of the eyes in a white whale , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[25]  Ralph L. Holloway,et al.  Brain, Evolution of , 2001 .

[26]  P. Gingerich,et al.  Origin of Whales in Epicontinental Remnant Seas: New Evidence from the Early Eocene of Pakistan , 1983, Science.

[27]  B. Pakkenberg,et al.  A Neurological Comparative Study of the Harp Seal (Pagophilus groenlandicus) and Harbor Porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) Brain , 2010, Anatomical record.

[28]  P. Nachtigall,et al.  Audiogram of a formerly stranded long-finned pilot whale (Globicephala melas) measured using auditory evoked potentials , 2010, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[29]  Suzana Herculano-Houzel,et al.  Not All Brains Are Made the Same: New Views on Brain Scaling in Evolution , 2011, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[30]  P. Manger,et al.  Quantitative analysis of neocortical gyrencephaly in African elephants (Loxodonta africana) and six species of cetaceans: Comparison with other mammals , 2012, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[31]  G. Roth,et al.  Evolution of the brain and intelligence , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  R. W. Baird,et al.  Diving and night-time behavior of long-finned pilot whales in the Ligurian Sea , 2002 .

[33]  T. F. Ladygina,et al.  [Multiple sensory projections in the dolphin cerebral cortex]. , 1978, Zhurnal vysshei nervnoi deiatelnosti imeni I P Pavlova.

[34]  P. Manger An examination of cetacean brain structure with a novel hypothesis correlating thermogenesis to the evolution of a big brain , 2006, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[35]  N. Emery Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  B. Pakkenberg,et al.  Neocortical and hippocampal neuron and glial cell numbers in the rhesus monkey , 2007, Anatomical record.

[37]  Christopher L. Asplund,et al.  Faster Scaling of Auditory Neurons in Cortical Areas Relative to Subcortical Structures in Primate Brains , 2013, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[38]  Harvey J Karten,et al.  Organization and evolution of the avian forebrain. , 2005, The anatomical record. Part A, Discoveries in molecular, cellular, and evolutionary biology.

[39]  O. Güntürkün,et al.  Mirror-Induced Behavior in the Magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of Self-Recognition , 2008, PLoS biology.

[40]  Patrick R Hof,et al.  Structure of the cerebral cortex of the humpback whale, Megaptera novaeangliae (Cetacea, Mysticeti, Balaenopteridae) , 2007, Anatomical record.

[41]  R. Williams,et al.  The control of neuron number. , 1988, Annual review of neuroscience.

[42]  Kleber Neves,et al.  The elephant brain in numbers , 2014, Front. Neuroanat..

[43]  L. Mukhametov,et al.  Unihemispheric slow-wave sleep in the Amazonian dolphin, Inia geoffrensis , 1987, Neuroscience Letters.

[44]  J. Kirkbride Lotus Systematics and Distribution , 2015 .

[45]  P. Manger Questioning the interpretations of behavioral observations of cetaceans: Is there really support for a special intellectual status for this mammalian order? , 2013, Neuroscience.

[46]  Comparative Stereology Studies of Brains from Marine Mammals , 2013 .

[47]  D. Reiss,et al.  Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  M. Webber,et al.  Marine Mammals of the World , 1972 .

[49]  B. Pakkenberg,et al.  Neocortical neuron number in humans: Effect of sex and age , 1997, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[50]  L. Herman What Laboratory Research has Told Us about Dolphin Cognition , 2010 .

[51]  P. Morgane,et al.  The limbic lobe of the dolphin brain: a quantitative cytoarchitectonic study. , 1982, Journal fur Hirnforschung.

[52]  Wendi Fellner,et al.  Understanding of the concept of numerically "less" by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.

[53]  H J Gundersen,et al.  Non‐uniform systematic sampling in stereology* , 2000, Journal of microscopy.

[54]  Jacob Jelsing,et al.  The postnatal development of neocortical neurons and glial cells in the Göttingen minipig and the domestic pig brain , 2006, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[55]  J. Winn,et al.  Brain , 1878, The Lancet.

[56]  H. J. Jerison Animal intelligence as encephalization. , 1985, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[57]  M. M. Kamshilov [Organization and evolution]. , 1970, Zhurnal obshchei biologii.

[58]  P. Mundinger,et al.  Animal cultures and a general theory of cultural evolution , 1980 .

[59]  F. Delfour,et al.  Mirror image processing in three marine mammal species: killer whales (Orcinus orca), false killer whales (Pseudorca crassidens) and California sea lions (Zalophus californianus) , 2001, Behavioural Processes.

[60]  G. Leuba,et al.  A quantitative study of neuronal and glial numerical density in the visual cortex of the bottlenose dolphin: Evidence for a specialized subarea and changes with age , 1986, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[61]  H J Gundersen,et al.  The efficiency of systematic sampling in stereology and its prediction * , 1987, Journal of microscopy.

[62]  R. Furutani Laminar and cytoarchitectonic features of the cerebral cortex in the Risso's dolphin (Grampus griseus), striped dolphin (Stenella coeruleoalba), and bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) , 2008, Journal of anatomy.

[63]  J. Lilly Vocal Mimicry in Tursiops: Ability to Match Numbers and Durations of Human Vocal Bursts , 1965, Science.

[64]  D. W. Rice,et al.  Marine mammals of the world, systematics and distributuion , 1998 .

[65]  J. Teilmann,et al.  Time allocation and diving behaviour of harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) in Danish and adjacent waters , 2023, J. Cetacean Res. Manage..

[66]  Dolphin cognition , 2004, Current Biology.

[67]  W. Hendelman The Limbic Lobe , 2000 .

[68]  L. Herman,et al.  Bottle-nosed dolphin: double-slit pupil yields equivalent aerial and underwater diurnal acuity. , 1975, Science.

[69]  H. Gundersen,et al.  The efficiency of systematic sampling in stereology — reconsidered , 1999, Journal of microscopy.

[70]  B. Pakkenberg,et al.  An efficient method for estimating the total number of neurons in rat brain cortex , 1990, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[71]  R. Payne,et al.  Songs of Humpback Whales , 1971, Science.

[72]  B. Pakkenberg,et al.  A postmortem study of the corpus callosum in the common minke whale (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) , 2011 .

[73]  O. Güntürkün,et al.  Neuron numbers in sensory cortices of five delphinids compared to a physeterid, the pygmy sperm whale , 2005, Brain Research Bulletin.

[74]  L. Marino Convergence of Complex Cognitive Abilities in Cetaceans and Primates , 2002, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[75]  S H Ridgway,et al.  Corpus callosum size in delphinid cetaceans. , 1994, Brain, behavior and evolution.