Who Are the Noisiest Neighbors in the Hood? Using Error Analyses to Study the Acquisition of Letter-Position Processing

This research examines the acquisition of letter-position processing. Study 1 investigated letter-position processing in Grades 1–6 and adult readers, using the occurrence of specific error types as the outcome measure. Between Grades 1 and 2, there was a shift from making more other-word to making more letter-position errors. This shift was a function of reading proficiency, not of years of reading instruction. Based on the multiple-route model of reading development (Grainger, Lété, Bertand, Dufau, & Ziegler, 2012), we argue that the fact that children make fewer other-word errors (i.e., mostly letter-identity errors) opens up the opportunity for them to make “the more advanced” letter-position errors. Finally, skilled adult readers still made fewer letter-position errors than typical readers in Grade 6, suggesting that the acquisition process is not finalized by the end of primary school. In Study 2, we directly compared letter-position processing with letter-identity processing. Thirty children in Grade 3 and 30 children in Grade 4 read aloud words with and without higher-frequency distractors. Children more often misread a word with a higher-frequency distractor than without such a distractor and this effect was stronger for below-average than for above-average readers. Converging with the results of Study 1, we found that a letter-position distractor is more disruptive than a letter-identity distractor. These results confirm that the acquisition of letter-position processing lags behind of that of letter-identity processing. The results are discussed within the framework of the Lexical Tuning Hypothesis (Castles, Davis, Cavalot, & Forster, 2007), which stresses the importance of feedback between letter (identity and position) coding and (developing) orthographic representations.

[1]  Colin J Davis,et al.  The spatial coding model of visual word identification. , 2010, Psychological review.

[2]  S. Kohnen,et al.  Assessment of lexical and non-lexical spelling in students in Grades 1–7 , 2015 .

[3]  R. Baayen,et al.  Analyzing Reaction Times , 2010 .

[4]  N. Friedmann,et al.  What Can Reduce Letter Migrations in Letter Position Dyslexia , 2014 .

[5]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[6]  Andrew V. Papachristos,et al.  Neighborhood Effects on Street Gang Behavior , 2005 .

[7]  C. Whitney How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  N. Friedmann,et al.  Developmental letter position dyslexia. , 2007, Journal of neuropsychology.

[9]  Morag Stuart,et al.  Children's printed word database: continuities and changes over time in children's early reading vocabulary. , 2010, British journal of psychology.

[10]  V. Csépe,et al.  Adaptive specialization in position encoding while learning to read. , 2017, Developmental science.

[11]  S. Kohnen,et al.  The Letter-Sound Test (LeST): a reliable and valid comprehensive measure of grapheme–phoneme knowledge , 2015 .

[12]  Phyllis Mindell Dyslexia and sequential order errors in reading , 1978 .

[13]  N. Friedmann,et al.  Letter Position Dyslexia , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[14]  Genevieve McArthur,et al.  When ‘slime’ becomes ‘smile’: Developmental letter position dyslexia in English , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Genevieve McArthur,et al.  A Test of Everyday Reading Comprehension (TERC) , 2013 .

[16]  T. R. Jordan,et al.  Children and adults both see 'pirates' in 'parties': letter-position effects for developing readers and skilled adult readers. , 2015, Developmental science.

[17]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Phonetic Biases in Voice Key Response Time Measurements , 2002 .

[18]  Anne Castles,et al.  Pirates at parties: letter position processing in developing readers. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[19]  P. Fridenberg SPECIFIC READING DISABILITY —STREPHOSYMBOLIA , 1928 .

[20]  S. Andrews Lexical Retrieval and Selection Processes: Effects of Transposed-Letter Confusability , 1996 .

[21]  Genevieve McArthur,et al.  Assessing the basic components of reading: A revision of the Castles and Coltheart test with new norms , 2009 .

[22]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .

[23]  Annette Bennett An analysis of errors in word recognition made by retarded readers. , 1942 .

[24]  Linnea C. Ehri,et al.  Grapheme-phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read words in English , 1998 .

[25]  A. Hesketh,et al.  Assessment of Comprehension and Expression (6-11) , 2001 .

[26]  Piers L. Cornelissen,et al.  Letter‐position encoding and dyslexia , 2005 .

[27]  Dennis Norris,et al.  Reading through a noisy channel: why there's nothing special about the perception of orthography. , 2012, Psychological review.

[28]  M. Critchley The Dyslexic Child , 1970 .

[29]  Charles A. Perfetti,et al.  Reading Ability: Lexical Quality to Comprehension , 2007 .

[30]  Manuel Perea,et al.  There is no clam with coats in the calm coast: Delimiting the transposed-letter priming effect , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[31]  E. Marinus,et al.  Australian comparison data for the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE) , 2013 .

[32]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Evidence for multiple routes in learning to read , 2012, Cognition.

[33]  E. Boder Developmental Dyslexia: a Diagnostic Approach Based on Three Atypical Reading‐spelling Patterns , 1973 .

[34]  Shinichi Nakagawa,et al.  A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed‐effects models , 2013 .

[35]  Jonathan S. Lefcheck,et al.  piecewiseSEM: Piecewise structural equation modelling in r for ecology, evolution, and systematics , 2015, 1509.01845.

[36]  N. Friedmann,et al.  Letter Position Dyslexia in Arabic: From Form to Position , 2012, Behavioural neurology.

[37]  Linnea C. Ehri,et al.  Reconceptualizing the Development of Sight Word Reading and Its Relationship to Recoding , 1992 .

[38]  Chris Davis,et al.  Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers: masked priming effects. , 2007, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[39]  Chris Davis,et al.  Neighbourhood Effects on Masked Form Priming in Developing Readers , 1999 .

[40]  S. Kohnen,et al.  The locus of impairment in English developmental letter position dyslexia , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[41]  D. Share Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition , 1995, Cognition.

[42]  Paul Schedl,et al.  The locus of , 1984 .

[43]  P. Brockhoff,et al.  lmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package) , 2014 .